Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Effective Public Engagement with Scholarly Research: A Case Study of March Mammal Madness, Study notes of Communication

The importance of scholars effectively disseminating knowledge beyond academic disciplines and institutions through various strategies such as science communication, knowledge translation, and public engagement. The case study focuses on March Mammal Madness, a public engagement platform that uses social media, storytelling, and community building to engage audiences in science. The document also discusses the benefits of such activities for scholars, especially those from historically excluded demographics, and provides insights into effective communication strategies and platforms.

Typology: Study notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/12/2022

ekavaria
ekavaria 🇺🇸

4.4

(38)

262 documents

1 / 21

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
AJHB Hackademics: Public Translation of Science
1
Integrative Approaches
to Dispersing Science: A
Case Study of March
Mammal Madness
Carlos Eduardo G. Amorim1, Mauna
Dasari2, Lara Durgavich3, Katie Hinde4*,
Marc Kissel5, Kristi L. Lewton6, Tisa
Loewen4
1Department of Biology, California State University;
2Department of Biological Sciences, University of
Notre Dame; 3Department of Human Evolutionary
Biology, Harvard University; 4School of Human
Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State
University; 5Department of Anthropology,
Appalachian State University; 6Department of
Integrative Anatomical Sciences, Keck School of
Medicine, University of Southern California
ABSTRACT
Objectives: Public engagement is
increasingly viewed as an important pillar of
scientific scholarship. For early career and
established scholars, however, navigating
the mosaic landscape of public education
and science communication, noted for rapid
“ecological” succession, can be daunting.
Moreover, academics are characterized by
diverse skills, motivations, values,
positionalities, and temperaments that may
differentially incline individuals to particular
public translation activities.
Methods: Here we briefly contextualize
engagement activities within a scholarly
portfolio, describe the use of one public
education program March Mammal
Madness to highlight approaches to
science communication, and explore
essential elements and practical
considerations for creating and sustaining
outreach pursuits in tandem with other
scholarly activities.
Results: March Mammal Madness, an
annual simulated tournament of living and
fossil animal taxa, has reached hundreds of
thousands of learners since 2013. This
program has provided a platform to
communicate research findings from biology
and anthropology and showcase numerous
scholars in these fields. March Mammal
Madness has leveraged tournament devices
to intentionally address topics of climate
change, capitalist environmental
degradation, academic sexism, and racist
settler-colonialism. The tournament,
however, has also perpetuated implicit
biases that need disrupting.
Conclusions: By embracing reflexive, self-
interrogative, and growth attitudes, the
tournament organizers iteratively refine and
improve this public science education
program to better align our activities with
our values and goals. Our experiences with
March Mammal Madness suggest that
dispersing science is most sustainable when
we combine ancestral adaptations for
cooperation, community, and story-telling
with good-natured competition in the
context of shared experiences and shared
values.
Keywords: SciComm; Public Engagement;
Values-Based Scholarship; Growth Attitudes
This is the submitted version of the following article:
Amorim CEG, Dasari M, Durgavich L, Hinde K,
Kissel M, Lewton KL, Loewen T. 2021. Integrative
Approaches to Dispersing Science: A Case Study of
March Mammal Madness. American Journal of
Human Biology. e23659. DOI: 10.1002/ajhb.23659,
which has been published in final form at
onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajhb.23659 as
part of a special #Hackademics issue in AJHB on
navigating academia guest edited by Profs. Cara
Ocobock and Christopher Lynn. This article may be
used for non-commercial purposes in accordance
with Wiley Terms and Conditions for Use of Self-
Archived Versions.
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa
pfd
pfe
pff
pf12
pf13
pf14
pf15

Partial preview of the text

Download Effective Public Engagement with Scholarly Research: A Case Study of March Mammal Madness and more Study notes Communication in PDF only on Docsity!

Integrative Approaches

to Dispersing Science: A

Case Study of March

Mammal Madness

Carlos Eduardo G. Amorim^1 , Mauna Dasari^2 , Lara Durgavich^3 , Katie Hinde^4 *, Marc Kissel^5 , Kristi L. Lewton^6 , Tisa Loewen^4 (^1) Department of Biology, California State University; (^2) Department of Biological Sciences, University of Notre Dame; 3 Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University; 4 School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University; 5 Department of Anthropology, Appalachian State University; 6 Department of Integrative Anatomical Sciences, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California ABSTRACT Objectives : Public engagement is increasingly viewed as an important pillar of scientific scholarship. For early career and established scholars, however, navigating the mosaic landscape of public education and science communication, noted for rapid “ecological” succession, can be daunting. Moreover, academics are characterized by diverse skills, motivations, values, positionalities, and temperaments that may differentially incline individuals to particular public translation activities. Methods : Here we briefly contextualize engagement activities within a scholarly portfolio, describe the use of one public education program— March Mammal Madness— to highlight approaches to science communication, and explore essential elements and practical considerations for creating and sustaining outreach pursuits in tandem with other scholarly activities. Results : March Mammal Madness, an annual simulated tournament of living and fossil animal taxa, has reached hundreds of thousands of learners since 2013. This program has provided a platform to communicate research findings from biology and anthropology and showcase numerous scholars in these fields. March Mammal Madness has leveraged tournament devices to intentionally address topics of climate change, capitalist environmental degradation, academic sexism, and racist settler-colonialism. The tournament, however, has also perpetuated implicit biases that need disrupting. Conclusions : By embracing reflexive, self- interrogative, and growth attitudes, the tournament organizers iteratively refine and improve this public science education program to better align our activities with our values and goals. Our experiences with March Mammal Madness suggest that dispersing science is most sustainable when we combine ancestral adaptations for cooperation, community, and story-telling with good-natured competition in the context of shared experiences and shared values. Keywords : SciComm; Public Engagement; Values-Based Scholarship; Growth Attitudes This is the submitted version of the following article: Amorim CEG, Dasari M, Durgavich L, Hinde K, Kissel M, Lewton KL, Loewen T. 2021. Integrative Approaches to Dispersing Science: A Case Study of March Mammal Madness. American Journal of Human Biology. e23659. DOI: 10.1002/ajhb.23659, which has been published in final form at onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajhb.23659 as part of a special #Hackademics issue in AJHB on navigating academia guest edited by Profs. Cara Ocobock and Christopher Lynn. This article may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance with Wiley Terms and Conditions for Use of Self- Archived Versions.

INTRODUCTION

As researchers and educators, 21st Century scholars are increasingly expected to effectively disperse knowledge beyond our academic disciplines and institutional classrooms (Habibi & Salim, 2021; Hinde et al., 2021; Hotez, 2020) in parallel with expanding expectations of research- intensive institutions (Crow & Dabars, 2015). Depending on the activities, audience, and purpose, dispersing scholarly research can involve science communication (“SciComm”), knowledge translation, public engagement with science, public outreach, and/or informal science education (Raban & Gordon, 2021; Curran et al., 2011; Esmail et al., 2020). Funding agencies, including the National Science Foundation and the Wellcome Trust, have identified public engagement as a core aspect of research. The COVID-19 pandemic has motivated calls for the National Institutes of Health to adopt a “Public Engagement” criterion among their existing criteria for Significance, Investigator, Innovation, Approach, and Environment (Woodruff & Woodruff, 2021). Even before the COVID- 19 pandemic was declared, World Health Organization Director-General Dr Tedros A. Ghebreyesus identified earning public trust and building scientific literacy as vital for confronting the current “infodemic” on social media and addressing urgent health challenges in the next decade (2020). Contributing to the public translation of our science can be deeply rewarding for scholars. Although academic writing increasingly affords opportunities for researchers to communicate aspects of our identity, philosophies, and values, public translation affords broader opportunities to manifest our values, identities, and voices. As scholars, our professional activities are fundamentally influenced by our foundational philosophies, cultural values, and individual perspectives shaped by our lived experiences (Fig. 1).To the extent that we intentionally anchor our academic portfolios to these aspects of ourselves as individuals embedded within our communities and societies, we can intentionally build and guide our research programs, curricular offerings, professional service, and public outreach in light of our personal vision and professional mission. Further, we can embrace strategies and tactics that harmoniously reflect our individual attributes, especially temperament and positionality, while developing and expanding our visualization, story-telling, and other communication skill sets (Botsis et al., 2020; Hinde et al., 2021; Keller & Limaye, 2020; Cooke et al., 2017). Tactics for dispersing our science can expand beyond the restricted audiences of conventional academia to “broadcast” approaches with the potential to reach wide public audiences (Fig. 1). Indeed science communication can be a vital activity for finding meaning and purpose as a scholar in an academy increasingly characterized by conspicuous production (Alvesson et al., 2017; Knights & Clarke, 2014). Such activities may be especially resonant for scholars from historically excluded and contemporarily marginalized demographics and identities, and should be highly-valued in evaluation (Schell et al., 2020). Moreover, rather than a tradeoff between research performance and public engagement, these activities can be mutually enhancing (Kassab, 2019). Despite the recognition of the importance of public translation of research, identifying appropriate methods for disseminating our science remains a formidable challenge. Conceptually “our research” can refer to the specific data we collect and analyze,

excited to contribute for this special Hackademics issue of the American Journal of Human Biology with more topical focus. Importantly, March Mammal Madness has not only allowed us to publicly translate the science of our academic subfields but we have been able to navigate our approach to the tournament specifically, and our professional activities more broadly, in accordance with our deeper philosophies and values. Moreover, our contributions to March Mammal Madness are only one aspect of diversified portfolios (Table 1), that are necessary to achieve our individual goals for message, audience, and impact. Table 1. Additional public translation and science outreach activities (selected) of MMM scientist-narrators and co-authors on the present manuscript. “Venue” Audience Format (Role) Access Contributors Skype-a-Scientist K-12 Students, Other Learners Live, Virtual Conversation (Expert Guest) Teacher- Organized; Group- Organized (Free) MD, LD, MK ComSciCon General Public Festival (Organizer) Ticket-Holders (Free) MD TED Talk Attendees, General Public In Person, Recorded (Presenter) Ticket-Holders (Live, Fee); Internet (Recording, Free) LD, KH American Museum of Natural History General Public In Person (Presenter) Workshop Enrollment (Fee) KH Cleveland Museum of Natural History General Public In Person (Presenter) Ticket-Holders (Fee) KH Ologies; Just the Zoo of Us; Brains On General Public Podcasts (Interviewee) Internet-Enabled, Media Device (Free) LD, MK, KLL Hominin Advent Calendar General Public Digital Advent Calendar (Creator) Internet (Free) MK Letters to a Pre-Scientist Grade 5- 10 Pen Pal Letters (Correspondent) Teacher- Organized (Free) KLL Camp Cardiac High School Students In Person (Presenter) Summer Camp (Fee) KLL Meet-A-Scientist at the Public Library Community Members In Person (Presenter) Public (Free) MD Bioanthropology News General Public Facebook Page (Administrator) Public (Free) MK Leaders through Education, Action and Hope (LEAH) Project Boston-area youth, underrepresented in STEM STEM Career Guidance (Panelist) Student Internship (Paid) MD BABIES General Public Documentary Series (Featured Scientist) Netflix Subscribers (Fee) KH

Darwin Day, North Carolina Museum of Natural Sciences Community Members Festival (Volunteer) Public (Free) MD Girl Scouts Field Biology Camp Girl Scouts (K-12) Camp (Facilitator) Leader- Organized (Fee) LD AAAS/DoSER Science for Seminaries Seminary Programs Science Curricula Materials (Advisor) Institution- Organized (AAAS Materials Free) MK Lincoln Park Zoo General Public Visitor Experience (Ambassador) Zoo Visitors (Free) MD MARCH MAMMAL MADNESS Annually since 2013, the simulated, single- elimination tournament March Mammal Madness, presents empirically-informed, narratively-delivered descriptions of what could happen during encounters among animals resulting in fatal/injurious, withdrawal, and other eliminating outcomes. After the outcomes are determined by a random number generator, scientist- narrators then craft a play-by-play narrative of the encounter from the scholarly literature (Hinde et al., 2021). Play-by-plays of the animal “encounters” are initially delivered by scientist-narrators as though being described in real time “live” on Twitter with embedded citations and links to the scholarly literature. These tweets are subsequently organized into collections (Storify 2013-2016, Wakelet 2017-2021, Twitter 2021), “reported” by MC Marmot and the Rodent Roundtable (YouTube 2017- 2021), and summarized into sports blurbs with content and links posted across multiple platforms, including a dedicated Facebook page, ASU March Mammal Madness LibGuide (Perry et al., 2017). These resources are also posted directly in a shared Google Drive Folder that is directly accessible by thousands of educators (2020- 2021). The creative team behind March Mammal Madness includes librarians, artists, life scientists, conservationists, graphic designers, sports-writers, social scientists, puppeteers, and educators working cooperatively to develop and deliver this open educational resource in English (since 2013) and Spanish (since 2020). Many tournament materials are now archived and freely available in the ASU March Mammal Madness KEEP collection (Perry & Hinde, 2020). In tandem with the tournament, since 2015 the American Society of Mammalogists have tweeted photos of tournament taxa from their photo collection and since 2017 Oxford University Press has organized an online special issue of freely available articles featuring tournament taxa. Initially created as a lark for “Biologist Twitter,” K-12 and college educators spontaneously adopted March Mammal Madness with their learners and represent the best-studied tournament users within the broader MMM community. For years, educators informally volunteered feedback via Tweets and email that were discussed among MMM organizers. In 2018, we launched pre-tournament surveys and in 2020 added post-tournament surveys to more systematically understand how and where educators are using the tournament with their learners. From these surveys we now understand that March Mammal Madness is distributed to hundreds of thousands of students (N>245,000 in 2019) proportionally across geographic regions in

Roar in 2013, 2017, and 2020 with Gorilla gorilla securing the championship in 2020, the first non-Carnivora or non-Artiodactyl Champion. To celebrate this victory, instead of going to Disney World, Anthropology News invited MMM Art Director Charon Henning to create a coloring page for a special summer issue (Henning, 2020). The inclusion of diverse primate species across multiple tournament years has enabled narrators to describe a multitude of primate adaptations including mating strategies, parental care, diet and digestion, foraging, predator detection and mobbing, primate predation, coloration, symbiosis, grooming behavior, home ranges, fallback foods, behavioral plasticity, sources of mortality, and ethnoprimatology. At times, these topics are contextualized in a comparative perspective that speaks to an evolutionary history of shared and divergent human behaviors. Scientist-narrators frequently emphasize primate conservation and the anthropogenic sources of population declines including habitat loss/degradation ( Tarsius tumpara vs. Macaca nigra , 2015), the wildlife trade ( Nycticebus javanicus vs. Lynx pardinus , 2015), and hunting ( Mandrillus sphinx vs. Ifrita kowaldi , 2021). In recent years, scientist-narrators have discussed how wild game can be economically and nutritionally important to locals so centering community collaborations is essential for conservation efforts to be sustainable. Moreover, scientist-narrators now intentionally use the term “game” to describe meat from wild animals, regardless of geographic location (Unigwe, 2017). The COVID-19 pandemic elevated the urgency for public understanding of zoonotic disease transmission and public health messaging. Indeed, the 2020 tournament bracket released just hours before the general public first learned of community spread in the United States at a long-term care facility in King County, WA. A round one match-up, Gorilla gorilla vs. Loa loa (a filarial nematode) provided an opportunity to discuss risk of zoonotic disease transmission in the context of ecotourism (Gouteux et al., 1989). Until shoved off the battlefield by the gorilla in the 2020 Final Roar, the gopher tortoise ( Gopherus polyphemus ) consistently outlasted opponents by SHELLtering-in- place. In 2021, audiences were left with a cliffhanger after the first round when a chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) was bitten by the ultimately defeated white-winged vampire ( Diaemus youngi ) wondering whether the chimpanzee could now be incubating an emergent infectious disease (Villena et al., 2018). These narrative devices stimulate “spectator” speculation and the high number of scientists who participate in playing March Mammal Madness typically contribute their expertise within speculative discussions. In this way, information exchange extends far beyond the “official” narration tweets of the scientist-narrators while facilitating expanded networks and more connections among scientists and non-scientists. Tournament players often have strong taxonomic preferences shaping their bracket predictions. This is as evident for scientists as the general public, especially when scientists’ study species are in the bracket. The “Of Myths and Monsters” Division in 2021 featured species whose Latin binomial or common English name made reference to mythical entities. Both Pan troglodytes and Mandrillus sphinx were in this division (along with fan-favorite and non-mammal combatant harpy eagle, Harpia harpyja ). In the week between the bracket drop and launch of the tournament battles, speculations about a chimpanzee vs. mandrill match-up, and which could

possibly defeat the harpy eagle, generated intense Twitter discussions. Relative body mass, canine length, fight style, and “smarts,” featured prominently in these dialogues, with one primatologist’s summation “chimpanzees are terrifying.” Battles with highly contentious fan investment exert notable pressure for scientist-narrators to deliver a play-by-play that is narratively suspenseful and concluded legitimately. Toward these ends, rich natural history descriptions are particularly invaluable for scientific story-telling (Ramsay & Teichroeb, 2019). Interestingly, the empirical literature provided no information on the behavioral interactions of wild-living mandrills and chimpanzees, despite some sympatry in their population distributions and shared dietary preferences. In the battle play-by-play, an adult male chimpanzee accompanied by a subadult male encountered a lone male mandrill terrestrially foraging for fallen figs in a forest fragment in Gabon (Tutin, 1999). The narrative described how chimpanzees hunt monkeys, smaller than adult male mandrills, with nutritional preferences for brains (Gilby & Wawrzyniak, 2018), the larger body mass of the chimpanzee vs. the larger canine size of the mandrill (Setchell, 2016), and situated an extensive, back-and-forth mutually assessing side-eye until… the chimpanzee climbed the tree to the nest of a crowned hawk-eagle to snarf some nestlings before a flying attack by the parent raptor sent the chimpanzee careening out of the tree and withdrawing from the MMM field of battle (Takahata et al., 1984). In one fell swoop, pun intended, the battle narration addressed both the likely disinclination of a chimpanzee to tangle with a mandrill and the probable outcome of a chimpanzee encounter with a harpy eagle. A disappointing outcome for many players, that even chimpanzee researchers acknowledged was grounded in realism (Figure 2). Figure 2. Dr. Kris Sabbi, researcher on the Kibale Chimpanzee Project, comment quote-tweeting the 2021 Sweet Sixteen ‘Chimpanzee vs. Mandrill’ battle outcome tweet. Paleoanthropology in March Mammal Madness Fossil taxa have frequently been featured in March Mammal Madness in dedicated divisions (Fossil Mammals 2014; Antecessor 2018) or in adaptation-centered divisions of extant and fossil species (Sexy Beasts, 2015; Of Myths and Monsters 2021). Even more than among extant mammals, fossil primates are notably featured here (N=8/35, 23%) with 50% of these species being hominins (N=4; Australopithecus afarensis , Australopithecus sediba , Homo neanderthalensis , Homo floresiensis ). MMM narratives about fossil primates have generally been contextualized within method and theory in paleontology for reconstructing species’ paleobiology, paleoenvironments, and paleoclimate (Cohen et al., 2016). In 2014, to introduce the Fossil Mammal Division, and first MMM paleo-combatants, the scientist- narrator provided introductory tweets on the comparative method (Harvey & Pagel,

  1. to explain how paleontologists infer or reconstruct behavior and body size in extinct taxa (e.g., Ross et al., 2002). Battles featuring fossil hominins have highlighted adaptations to body size dimorphism, diet,

some morphometric and behavioral inferences remain somewhat tenuous. In this way we aim to showcase that the scientific endeavor is an iterative process and perhaps inspire the next generation of aspiring scientists to pursue unanswered questions. Genetics & Genomics in March Mammal Madness In 2016, Prof Anne Stone established an ongoing genetics theme within March Mammal Madness. The genetics team contributes tweets with evolutionary, genetics, genomics, and phylogenetic facts about each year’s combatants, in parallel to the main battle play-by-play tweets. Featured content includes the genetic basis of local adaptation in primates, the impact of human activities on the genetic diversity of natural populations, and the recovery of DNA molecules from unusual sources such as feces, museum specimens, and bones. For instance, in 2018 ( Pseudaelurus quadridentatus vs. Archaeoindris fontoynontii ) MMM featured an ancient DNA article about extinct sloth lemur genera, where phylogenetic relationships between extinct and extant lemur species were reconstructed (Karanth et al., 2005). Topics such as “ancient DNA” have a strong public appeal and often spark public engagement, and tournament players interact with these tweets with questions or comments. MMM genetics tweets also feature articles about the molecular basis of primate traits, often contextualized into known primate phylogenies. For instance, work featured in 2020 ( Gorilla gorilla vs. Pristis pristis, Elite Trait, 2020 ) shows how the analysis of the genetic variation in the FOXP2 gene across great apes can offer insights into the evolution of communication in primates (Staes et al., 2017). FOXP2 is associated with human language disorders and it is thought to be involved with the evolution of speech and language in humans (Enard et al., 2002). A recurrent theme within MMM genetics tweets is the distribution of trichromatic vision in primates and the genetic variation in genes coding for opsin proteins (light-sensitive proteins involved in vision) across the tree of life. In 2021 alone, the genetics team’s tweets featured five different articles on opsin genetic variation, one on primates (including that year’s combatant, the red ruffed lemur, Varecia rubra ) and others on non-primate species, such as fish (e.g., the dragonfish) and even in the eyeless Hydra vulgaris (Cnidaria). Together, these tweets highlight characteristics and genes that humans share with other species – primates or not – shedding light onto the evolutionary fact of common descent among primates in particular and across the tree of life more generally. When crafting genetics tweets, the genetics team follows similar strategies to that of the scientist-narrators. Tweets include hashtags, accessible language, an image (reproduced from cited literature), and a peer-reviewed article (often from open-access sources). Relevant genetic information about a given combatant is summarized in a single tweet posted in orchestration with the principle battle narration. This format allows players to quickly interact with the genetics content and, at the same time, follow the battle narration. Cited literature includes diverse themes within genetics and genomics, including classic karyotype studies from decades ago to more recent next-generation sequencing studies. Since tournament combatants are selected from all continents, genetics tweets often feature scientific research done by local scientists, amplifying the work of scholars from outside the US and Western Europe.

Improving March Mammal Madness Each year, the creative team behind March Mammal Madness has endeavored to refine and improve the tournament in multiple dimensions responding to formal and informal feedback, adopting better accessibility practices, and addressing legacies of exclusion within society, academia, and STEM communities. These efforts have been ongoing and team members are committed to a growth mentality, recognizing that we have at times made mistakes, perpetuated biases, and neglected essential priorities. In 2016, our “March Mammal Madness Standard Operating Procedures” were transformed from oral tradition to a written, annually- updated document that formally guides official tournament tweets, roles, and priorities, in combination with detailed explanations for procedures and recommendations. Initially focused on celebrating mammals and the natural world, with sub-themes of conservation priorities and acceptance of evolutionary theory, the tournament has expanded elements to support more general scientific literacy and knowledge connection. Each year, combatant taxa are selected in consideration of taxonomic diversity, global representation, and ecosystem breadth and featuring a mix of familiar and unfamiliar species (Hinde et al., 2021). ASU Librarian Anali Perry’s curation of reliable, freely available internet resources into a dedicated online Library Guide updated annually (Perry et al., 2017) enhances player navigation of the mosaic digital information landscape (Hinde et al., 2021). Scientist-narrators often cite and link scholarly publications, but even gold- standard open access articles are situated “behind a paywall of jargon” (Hinde et al., 2021), so narrations routinely also link to publicly-accessible, responsibly-described media coverage of research findings. Given the large number of educators and learners using the tournament, scientist-narrators are encouraged to cite and link to articles in ‘Frontiers for Kids’, an innovative Open Access journal in which scientific findings are made accessible to younger learners through effective language and illustration with oversight from kid editors and kid reviewers. Organizers design divisions to illustrate different selective pressures, species naming practices, and build bridges to the fields of humanities (Hinde et al., 2021). For example, some divisions are organized around adaptations shaped by the environment (Marine Mammals, 2014; Desert-Adapted Mammals, 2017), or adaptations in relation to social organization (Social Mammals, 2014) or sexual selection (Sexy Beasts, 2015). Divisions have been organized for animals in relation to parts of speech. For example, Adjective Mammals in 2017 featured species whose notable physical and/or behavioral characteristics, like the screaming hairy armadillo, were embedded in their common English names. Similarly, Mammals of the Nouns in 2016 dignified taxa named for their habitat. The humble rock hyrax and modest volcano rabbit were thereby humorously restyled into Hyrax of the Rock and Rabbit of the Volcano. Heraldry was involved. In 2015, we built connections to the Humanities through a Division of Mythical Mammals from around the world (and antiquity) and inversely in 2021, divisions of real animals named for myths and monsters (Sea Beasties; Of Myths & Monsters). To facilitate more effective integration of the tournament with Next Generation Science Standards, MMM introduced basic science lesson plans in 2018 to be used in tandem with the tournament and added advanced AP Biology permutations in 2019. In 2020, we introduced Language Arts and Visual Arts lesson plans in 2020 in harmony with

was immediately challenged by consolidated federal lawsuits from a coalition of plaintiff Tribes, conservation organizations, and a corporation. When battles were situated at tinajas, narrations showcased the cultural and environmental significance of the Bears Ears landscape and directed player attention to the concurrent legal case and ways to support protection efforts. March Mammal Madness has additionally used aspects of the tournament to confront sexism, industrialism, and racism. A battle between the mythical Greek Sphinx and Ratatoskr served as a discussion of Yet Another Mostly Male Meeting #YAMMM (Eisen, 2016) when the Norse squirrel god maliciously gossiped about how Egyptian Sphinx “organized a symposium on humanoid lions & only invited males!" because “stag parties linger” (Isbell et al., 2012). In the narration, Greek Sphinx and Ratatoskr then went back and forth discussing anthropomorphized lions in material culture including Maahes, Bastet and Sekmet (ancient Egypt), Apedemak (ancient Nubia), Narasimha (Hinduism), Manticore (Persian), and Apsasu (Mesopotamia), among others. Scientist- narrators highlight global warming, ecosystem deterioration, and habitat destruction due to extractive capitalism and links between many invasive species within the continuing legacies of imperialism and settler-colonialism At times, scientist- narrators explicitly contrast industrialized practices with thousands of years of sustainable land management by Indigenous communities (Ellis et al., 2021). Our annually-updated March Mammal Madness FAQ cautions players to not appropriate “totem animal” or use digital Blackface, with links to explanatory essays on these topics. Many of these elements have been adopted into the tournament due to MMM team members attentiveness to broader online conversations on justice, inclusivity, and equity as well as feedback from user communities. In this iterative process, community involvement and co-creation are two very essential components of decolonial “praxis”. While expanding tournament content and adopting new practices are important elements of improving March Mammal Madness, reflexivity on exclusionary elements are also essential for better science outreach activities. For example, in 2015 while aiming to disrupt Eurocentric classical mythology by additionally featuring mythical combatants from diverse cultures, we featured mythical combatants without verifying that such use of cultural mythology was appropriate and welcome or was appropriating and disrespectful. Similarly in 2021, Sea Beasties and Of Myths and Monsters Divisions featured species whose etymology derived from mythology. Some Latin binomials alluded to local traditional mythology, and review of the holotype description and media coverage did not report to what extent Indigenous communities collaborated in the assignment of their languages and cultural mythology to these taxonomic designations. Within Eurocentric academic worldviews, there is a prioritization for open data, transparent information, and knowledge as a public good; but these cultural attitudes are not universal and rather rest on a “specific epistemological position” (Bacevic & Muellerleile, 2018). In contrast, some philosophical worldviews consider knowledge as protected, earned and emplaced, and accepting the validity of these philosophical views is especially important for decolonizing science (see Tsosie et al., 2021). Similarly, white, Eurocentric approaches to science education and science communication are compromised by overlooking other experts

and other practices, undervaluing other knowledge, and thereby missing opportunities and audiences (Finlay et al., 2021). Another major deficiency of the March Mammal Madness model is that the game structure is such that when predicting outcomes, the probabilities are based on an individual combatant that represents a prime competitor of the species. For mammals, this means that featured combatants are most typically males resulting from differences in sexual selection pressures due to asymmetry of parental effort within our Class (but “YAY LACTATION!” - KH). This perpetuates male-centered biases. Broader taxonomic inclusion beyond mammals has allowed us to showcase species in which females are larger and more badass than males (e.g., green anaconda, harpy eagle) but more intentional and sustained efforts are needed. Effective, value-based science outreach requires substantial planning to effectively engage in collaborative discussions with intended user communities, adequately develop skills and materials, and to hopefully guard against unintended messaging. As scientists, to the extent that our dialogues about our areas of research are with like-minded scholars, we can often acquire some tunnel-vision in how we think and talk about our science. A tournament of simulated encounters of animal combatants is all well and fun, until one considers the political implications of animals as national symbols. In March Mammal Madness, combatant species are typically selected months in advance to facilitate artwork, background research, and pre-season hype. But in 2019, when Panthera tigris and Capra falconeri were included in the tournament because of their adaptations for saltation in the Jump Jump Division, geopolitical events unfolded to our great consternation. Lesson plan materials were already distributed to educators when border tensions between India and Pakistan precipitously escalated into airstrikes. At this point, an organizer recollected that the Bengal tiger and the markhor are the national animals of India and Pakistan, respectively. Fortuitously that year, outcomes of match-ups had already been determined and we knew the tiger and markhor were not slated to battle. This near miss of a political morass demonstrates the importance of thinking carefully about the potential for unintended messaging in science outreach. Moreover, large collaborative teams do not just mean “many hands, lighter loads,” but that there are many diverse perspectives brought to bear during discussions and planning. The numerous contributors to March Mammal Madness have been instrumental for sustaining a monumental annual undertaking. Growing Science Outreach March Mammal Madness is only one example of public engagement within a species-rich ecosystem of science communication. Within the scholarly literature, key primers guide decision trees for scholars developing SciComm campaigns (for example see: Cooke et al., 2017; Lundgren et al., 2020) while others highlight the difficulties in training scholars in SciComm (Ruebega et al., 2021; Baldwin-SoRelle & Swann, 2020). Lungren and colleagues (2020) recommend the “SMARTER” mnemonic for goal-setting when launching an outreach campaign or agenda. SMARTER— Specific, Measurable, Attainable, Relevant, Timebound, Effective, and Rewarding (Rubin, 2002)— facilitates a goal structure that informs the design, incorporates assessment tools, and embeds reflection of effort to reward ratios. In 2017, Cooke and colleagues prepared a thorough 16 - point guide, expanding topics to include how SciComm improves our science and the

  • Use inclusive science communication practices; reject exclusionary, marginalizing, and “otherizing” messaging
  • Develop guiding resources for users and protocols for team members Assess & Reassess the Effort
  • Measure traffic, stakeholder and user experiences, learning and attitudinal outcomes
  • Invite user ideas, suggestions, and critiques and refine accordingly, to manifest ‘descent with modification’ to the extent favored
  • Generate information for grant proposals, funding reports, manuscripts, annual evaluations, and job applications
  • Consider the benefits, opportunity costs, and tradeoffs for engaging in this work, periodically Remember the Purpose
  • Ensure efforts contribute to a personal and/or scholarly mission in harmony with one’s own vision, values, and philosophies In 2022, March Mammal Madness will celebrate the 10th annual simulated tournament, an exciting milestone never imagined by the original founder in 2013. By embracing reflexive, self-interrogative, and growth attitudes, the March Mammal Madness team has been able to iteratively refine and improve this public science education program to better align our activities with our values and goals while expanding the user community. Our experiences with March Mammal Madness suggest that dispersing science is most sustainable when we combine ancestral adaptations for cooperation, community, and story-telling with good-natured competition in the context of shared experiences and shared values (Hinde et al., 2021). We encourage our colleagues to center the fun and community in their public translation and science engagement efforts. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank each member of the March Mammal Madness team for their contributions to this ongoing public engagement program. In light of the topic areas of the present manuscript, we particularly appreciate Prof. Anne C. Stone for initiating and directing Team Genetics, thank the other genetics content contributors Melissa Wilson, Elinor Karlsson, Jesse Weber, and thank paleontologist contributor Brian Tanis. We thank Guest Editors Christopher Lynn and Cara Ocobock for the invitation to contribute to this special issue on Hackademics and Dr. Briana Pobinar and one anonymous reviewer for comments and suggestions that strengthened our article. AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS All authors were involved in drafting the manuscript and revising it critically for important intellectual content; given final approval of the version to be submitted; and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare for the content of this manuscript. DATA AVAILABILITY & ETHICS APPROVAL Summary and source data associated with the annual March Mammal Madness tournament for science education and

outreach are available in the ASU Research Data Repository (https://dataverse.asu.edu/dataverse/MarchM ammalMadness) with additional public materials curated in the ASU KEEP March Mammal Madness Collection (Perry and Hinde 2020). Surveys of educators were conducted after study design and procedures were evaluated for human subjects ethical review under Protocol STUDY "March Mammal Madness in the Classroom" by PI Katie Hinde approved 1/9/2018 by Arizona State University Institutional Review Board and Modification MOD00009767 approved 2/13/2019. CITATIONS Abrams, G., Bello, S. M., Di Modica, K., Pirson, S., & Bonjean, D. (2014). When Neanderthals used cave bear ( Ursus spelaeus ) remains: Bone retouchers from unit 5 of Scladina Cave (Belgium). Quaternary International , 326 , 274-

  1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2013.10. Alex, B. (2016). Is It Neander-TAL Or Neander- THAL? Discover. Retrieved from https://www.discovermagazine.com/planet- earth/is-it-neander-tal-or-neander-thal Alvesson, M., Gabriel, Y., & Paulsen, R. (2017). Return to meaning: A social science with something to say. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Antón, M. (2013). Sabertooth. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. Bacevic, J., & Muellerleile, C. (2018). The moral economy of open access. European Journal of Social Theory , 21 (2), 169-188. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Bader, A. C., Martin S., & Malhi, R. S. (2018). Politics of collaborative research with Indigenous communities: moving beyond the framework of community engagement. In R. C. O'Malley (Chair), Biological Anthropology and Dialogue with Diverse Publics Symposium. American Association of Physical Anthropology Meeting, Austin, TX. Retrieved from https://players.brightcove.net/53038991001/Byx STOH0_default/index.html?videoId= 001 Baldwin-SoRelle, C., & Swann, J. M. (2020). Scholarly bridges: Scicomm skill-building with student-created open educational resources. In K. D. Hoffman & A. Clifton (Eds.), Open Pedagogy Approaches. Retrieved from https://milnepublishing.geneseo.edu/openpedagog yapproaches/ Batt, S. (2009). Human attitudes towards animals in relation to species similarity to humans: A multivariate approach. Bioscience horizons , 2 (2), 180 - 190. DOI:10.1093/biohorizons/hzp Benton M. J. (2010). Studying function and behavior in the fossil record. PLoS Biol, 8 (3), e1000321. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio. Beyene, A. G., & Panescu, P. (2020). A community for Black chemists. Nature Chemistry, 12 , 988–
  2. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41557- 020 - 00572 - 3 Bezanson, M., & McNamara, A. (2019). The what and where of primate field research may be failing primate conservation. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 28 (4), 166-178. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan. Bock W. J., & von Wahlert G. (1965). Adaptation and the form-function complex. Evolution, 19 (3), 269 - 299. https://doi.org/10.2307/ Borgi, M., & Cirulli, F. (2016). Pet face: mechanisms underlying human-animal relationships. Frontiers in psychology, 7 , 298. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016. Botsis, T., Fairman, J. E., Moran, M. B., & Anagnostou, V. (2020). Visual storytelling enhances knowledge dissemination in biomedical science. Journal of biomedical informatics, 107 ,
  3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2020. Burgin, C. J., Colella, J. P., Kahn, P. L., & Upham, N. S. (2018). How many species of mammals are there? Journal of Mammalogy, 99 (1), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1093/jmammal/gyx Cohen, A., Campisano, C., Arrowsmith, R., Asrat, A., Behrensmeyer, A. K., Deino, A., … Zinaye, B. (2016). The Hominin Sites and Paleolakes Drilling Project: Inferring the environmental context of human evolution from eastern African rift lake deposits. Scientific Drilling , 21 , 1-16. https://doi.org/10.5194/sd- 21 - 1 - 2016 Canfield, K., & Menezes, S. (2020). The state of inclusive science communication: A landscape study. Kingston, RI: Metcalf Institute, University of Rhode Island.

(2012). Birds of a feather: Neanderthal exploitation of raptors and corvids. PloS ONE, 7 , e45927. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone. Ghebreyesus, T. A. (2020, January 13). Urgent health challenges for the next decade. World Health Organization. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/news-room/photo- story/photo-story-detail/urgent-health-challenges- for-the-next-decade Gilby, I. C., & Wawrzyniak, D. (2018). Meat eating by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): Effects of prey age on carcass consumption sequence. International Journal of Primatology, 39 , 127-140. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764- 018 - 0019 - 9 Gokhman, D., Agranat-Tamir, L., Housman, G., García-Pérez, R., Nissim-Rafinia, M., Nieves- Colón, M. A., … Carmel, L. (2017). Recent regulatory changes shaped human facial and vocal anatomy. bioRxiv. 106955; doi:10.1101/ accessed July 16 2021 www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/106955v Gouteux, J. P., Noireau, F., & Staak, C. (1989). The host preferences of Chrysops silacea and C. dimidiata (Diptera: Tabanidae) in an endemic area of Loa loa in the Congo. Annals of Tropical Medicine & Parasitology, 83 (2), 167-172. https://doi.org/10.1080/00034983.1989. Habibi, S. A., & Salim, L. (2021). Static vs. dynamic methods of delivery for science communication: A critical analysis of user engagement with science on social media. PLoS ONE, 16 , e0248507. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone. Hardy, K., Buckley, S., & Huffman, M. (2013). Neanderthal self-medication in context. Antiquity, 87 (337), 873-878. DOI: 10.1017/S0003598X Harvey, P. H., & Pagel M. D. (1991). The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Henning, C. (2020, June 19). Coloring for Anthropologists. Anthropology News. Retrieved from https://anthropology- news.org/articles/coloring-for-anthropologists/ Hinde, K., Amorim, C. E. G., Brokaw, A. F., Burt, N., Casillas, M. C., Chen, A., … Anderson, C. N. (2021). March Mammal Madness and the power of narrative in science outreach. eLife , 10, e65066. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife. Isbell, L. A., Young, T. P., & Harcourt, A. H. (2012). Stag parties linger: Continued gender bias in a female-rich scientific discipline. PLoS ONE, 7 , e49682. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone. Kachen, A., Krishen, A. S., Petrescu, M., Gill, R. D., & Peter, P. C. (2021). #MeToo, #MeThree, #MeFour: Twitter as community building across academic and corporate institutions. Psychology & Marketing, 38 , 455-469. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar. Kalogiannakis, M., Papadakis, S., & Zourmpakis, A. I. (2021). Gamification in science education. A systematic review of the literature. Education Sciences, 11 (1), 22. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci Kappel, K., & Holmen, S. J. (2019). Why science communication, and does it work? A taxonomy of science communication aims and a survey of the empirical evidence. Frontiers in Communication, 4 , 55. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2019. Karanth, K. P., Delefosse, T., Rakotosamimanana, B., Parsons, T. J., & Yoder, A. D. (2005). Ancient DNA from giant extinct lemurs confirms single origin of Malagasy primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 102 (14), 5090-

  1. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas. Kassab, O. (2019). Does public outreach impede research performance? Exploring the ‘researcher’s dilemma’ in a sustainability research center. Science and Public Policy, 46 (5), 710-720. https://doi.org/10.1093/scipol/scz Keller, A. B., & Limaye, V. S. (2020). Engaged science: Strategies, opportunities and benefits. Sustainability, 12 (19), 7854. https://doi.org/10.3390/su Kistler, L., Ratan, A., Godfrey, L. R., Crowley, B. E., Hughes, C. E., Lei, R., … Perry, G. H. (2015). Comparative and population mitogenomic analyses of Madagascar’s extinct, giant “subfossil” lemurs. Journal of Human Evolution, 79, 45 - 54. DOI:10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.06. Klar, S., Krupnikov, Y., Ryan, J. B., Searles, K., & Shmargad, Y. (2020). Using social media to promote academic research: Identifying the benefits of twitter for sharing academic work. PloS ONE, 15 , e0229446. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone. Knights, D., & Clarke, C. A. (2014). It’s a bittersweet symphony, this life: Fragile academic selves and

insecure identities at work_. Organization Studies, 35_ (3), 335-357. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Luo, T., Freeman, C., & Stefaniak, J. (2020). “Like, comment, and share”— Professional development through social media in higher education: A systematic review. Educational Technology Research and Development, 68 , 1659-1683. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423- 020 - 09790 - 5 Lundgren, L., Stofer, K. A., Hecht, K., & Williams, T. (2020). Using social media to engage communities with research: SMART social media— Planning for success. EDIS, 2020 (6), 1-

  1. http://doi.org/10.32473/edis-wc362- 2020 Márquez, M. C., & Porras, A. M. (2020). Science communication in multiple languages is critical to its effectiveness. Frontiers in Communication, 5 ,
  2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2020. Massarani, L., & Merzagora, M. (2014). Socially inclusive science communication. Journal of Science Communication, 13 (2), C01. https://doi.org/10.22323/2. McClain, C. R. (2019). Likes, comments, and shares of marine organism imagery on Facebook. PeerJ, 7 , e6795. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj. Mukhopadhyay, S., Kanakis, C., Golab, K., Hermelin, D., Crane, G. M., & Mirza, K. M. (2021). The network that never sleeps. Laboratory Medicine, 52 (4) , e83-e103. https://doi.org/10.1093/labmed/lmaa Ocobock, C., & Hawley, P. (2020). Science on tap: Effective public engagement or preaching to the choir? Journal of Science Communication, 19 (1), A04. https://doi.org/10.22323/2. Ortega, R. P. (2021). Black scientists gather to form communities and boost diversity in science. Nature Medicine, 27 , 756-758. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591- 021 - 01315 - 8 Perry, A., Tanner, R., Gohr, A., Hermer, J., Bonanni, M., & Hinde, K. (2017) ASU LibGuide March Mammal Madness, Arizona State University. Retrieved from https://libguides.asu.edu/MarchMammalMadness Perry, A., & Hinde, K. (2020). March Mammal Madness collection Library KEEP Repository, Arizona State University. Retrieved from https://hdl.handle.net/2286/R.2.C. Phillips, K. A., Bales, K. L., Capitanio, J. P., Conley, A., Czoty, P. W., ‘t Hart, B.A., … Nathanielsz, P. W. (2014). Why primate models matter. American Journal of Primatology, 76 (9), 801-827. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp. Pobiner, B. (2016). Accepting, understanding, teaching, and learning (human) evolution: Obstacles and opportunities. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 159 (561), 232-274. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa. Raban, D. R., & Gordon, A. (2021). Science communication: the intellectual structure of an evolving subject area. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/ 49_Science_Communication_The_Intellectual_St ructure_of_an_Evolving_Subject_Area_Pre-print Ramsay, M. S., & Teichroeb, J. A. (2019). Anecdotes in primatology: temporal trends, anthropocentrism, and hierarchies of knowledge. American Anthropologist, 121 (3), 680-693. https://doi.org/10.1111/aman. Reeve, M. A., & Partridge, M. (2017). The use of social media to combat research-isolation. Annals of the Entomological Society of America, 110 (5), 449 - 456. https://doi.org/10.1093/aesa/sax Ross, C. F., Lockwood, C. A., Fleagle, J. G., & Jungers, W. L. (2002). Adaptation and behavior in the primate fossil record. In J. M. Plavcan, R. F. Kay, W. L. Jungers & C. P. van Schaik (Eds.), Reconstructing Behavior in the Primate Fossil Record (pp.1-41). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Publishers. Rubega, M. A., Burgio, K. R., MacDonald, A. A. M., Oeldorf-Hirsch, A., Capers, R. S., & Wyss, R. (2021). Assessment by audiences shows little effect of science communication training. Science Communication, 43 (2), 139-169. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Rubin, R. S. (2002). Will the real SMART goals please stand up. The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, 39 (4), 26-27. Schell, C. J., Guy, C., Shelton, D. S., Campbell- Staton, S. C., Sealey, B. A., Lee, D. N., & Harris, N. C. (2020). Recreating Wakanda by promoting Black excellence in ecology and evolution. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 4 , 1285-1287. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559- 020 - 1266 - 7 Schipper, J., Chanson, J. S., Chiozza, F., Cox, N. A., Hoffmann, M., Katariya, V., ... & Young, B. E. (2008). The status of the world's land and marine mammals: diversity, threat, and knowledge.