Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Effects of Self-Instructional Training on Math and Printing in Retarded Children, Exams of Printing

A study conducted at the University of Notre Dame, where retarded children were trained using self-instructional methods to improve their attention and behavior during math and printing tasks. The study found that verbal instructional procedures facilitated the acquisition of new behaviors and increased appropriate responding in various situations. However, no previous studies had evaluated the effects of self-instructional training on distractible retarded children. The researchers used a 'storylike' instructional procedure, introduced distracting stimuli, and employed multiple exemplars of academic tasks. The results showed that children learned to verbalize self-instructional components without prompting and displayed a high frequency of self-instructional behavior in the generalization situation. Off-task behavior was also reduced after training.

What you will learn

  • How did off-task behavior change for the experimental children and the criterion comparison children?
  • What teaching method was used to assist children in recognizing situations for self-instruction?
  • How did the children perform on the math and printing tasks during the baseline and training conditions?
  • What was the frequency of self-instructional behavior displayed by the children in the transfer I and II settings?
  • What were the reasons suggested for the absence of performance change across tasks?

Typology: Exams

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/12/2022

bartolix
bartolix 🇬🇧

4.8

(17)

304 documents

1 / 17

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
JOURNAL
OF
APPLIED
BEHAVIOR
ANALYSIS
A
SELF-INSTRUCTIONAL
PACKAGE
FOR
INCREASING
ATTENDING
BEHAVIOR
IN
EDUCABLE
MENTALLY
RETARDED
CHILDREN
Louis
D.
BuRGIo,
THOMAS
L.
WHITMAN,
AND
MOSES
R.
JOHNSON
UNIVERSITY
OF
NOTRE
DAME
The
purpose
of
this
study
was
to
develop
a
self-instructional
package
that
would
aid
highly
distractible
retarded
children
in
increasing
their
attending
behavior
in
a
training
and
two
generalization
(a
one-to-one
and
a
classroom)
situations.
Three
untrained
sub-
jects
were
monitored
for
general
comparison
and
social
validation
purposes.
One
of
these
control
subjects
was
distractible
and
the
other
two
(criterion
comparison)
were
evaluated
as
not
having
attentional
problems.
A
multiple
baseline
design
was
employed
in
which
training
was
sequentially
introduced
across
subjects.
During
training,
the
ex-
perimental
subjects
were
taught
through
self-instruction
to
focus
their
attention
and
to
cope
with
two
tasks,
math
and
printing.
After
learning
the
self-instructions
the
subjects
were
systematically
and
sequentially
exposed
to
photo-slides
of
distracting
situations,
to
audio-distractors
composed
of
noisy
lunchroom
verbal
peer
interactions,
and
to
in
vivo
distractors
provided
by
kindergarten
children
playing
with
wooden
blocks
in
the
training
setting.
The
entire
training
procedure
was
handled
in
a
game-like
context
to
maintain
subject
interest
and
to
facilitate
generalization.
The
results
suggested
that
the
training
package
produced
direct
and
generalized
changes
in
self-instructional
behavior.
In
addi-
tion,
a
decrease
in
off-task
behavior
occurred
during
math,
printing,
and
also
during
a
phonics
program
in
the
one-to-one
and
classroom
situations.
However,
reliable
changes
in
academic
task
performance
were
not
observed.
Finally,
no
systematic
changes
on
any
of
the
dependent
measures
occurred
for
the
three
untrained
subjects.
DESCRIPTORS:
self-instruction,
attending
behavior,
academic
behavior,
generaliza-
tion,
retarded
children
Within
educational
settings,
teachers
recog-
nize
that
students
must
sustain
attention
to
school-related
materials
and
activities
if
learning
This
paper
is
based
on
a
thesis
completed
by
the
first
author
in
partial
fulfillment
of
the
requirements
for
the
master
of
arts
degree
in
psychology
at
the
University
of
Notre
Dame.
The
research
reported
herein
was
supported
by
a
National
Research
Train-
ing
Award
#HD07184-01.
Sections
of
this
paper
were
presented
at
the
annual
convention
of
the
Asso-
ciation
for
the
Advancement
of
Behavior
Therapy,
San
Francisco,
December,
1979.
The
authors
thank
Carol
Glass,
Kathy
Larsen,
John
Scibak,
Sandy
Levers,
and
Jim
Little
for
their
assistance
in
the
project.
Spe-
cial
thanks
go
to
John
Borkowski,
Ellen
Ryan,
Denny
Reid,
and
Tom
Merluzzi
for
their
helpful
comments
on
the
manuscript.
Thanks
are
also
due
to
Sr.
Ann
Fox
and
the
children
of
St.
Bavos
elementary
school
for
their
cooperation
throughout
the
project.
Reprints
are
available
from
Lou
Burgio
or
Tom
Whitman,
De-
partment
of
Psychology,
University
of
Notre
Dame,
Notre
Dame,
Indiana
46556.
is
to
occur.
Where
attentional
deficits
exist,
ini-
tial
emphasis
is
often
placed
on
the
teacher
for
ameliorating
this
problem.
Since
the
advent
of
behavior
modification
a
variety
of
techniques
have
been
developed
to
modify
distractible,
non-
attentive
behaviors
in
normal
and
retarded
popu-
lations,
including:
positive
reinforcement
of
in-
compatible
behaviors
(Alabiso,
1975;
Patterson,
1965;
Patterson,
Jones,
Whittier,
&
Wright,
1965;
Whitman,
Caponigri,
&
Mercurio,
1971),
time
out
procedures
(Johnson,
Whitman,
&
Bar-
toon-Noble,
1978),
and
aversive
stimulation
(Forehand
&
Baumeister,
1970;
Reardon
&
Bell,
1970).
Recently,
a
number
of behavioral
therapists
have
emphasized
the
need
to
switch
the
locus
of
control
for
an
individual's
behavior
from
exter-
nal
agents
(e.g.,
the
teacher)
to
the
individual
himself
(cf.
Kazdin,
1975).
They
argue
that
443
1980,
139
443-459
NUMBER
3
(FALL
1980)
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa
pfd
pfe
pff

Partial preview of the text

Download Effects of Self-Instructional Training on Math and Printing in Retarded Children and more Exams Printing in PDF only on Docsity!

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS

A SELF-INSTRUCTIONAL PACKAGE FOR INCREASING ATTENDING BEHAVIOR IN EDUCABLE MENTALLY RETARDED (^) CHILDREN Louis D. BuRGIo, THOMAS L. WHITMAN, AND MOSES R. JOHNSON

UNIVERSITY OF NOTRE DAME

The purpose of this study was to develop a self-instructional package that would aid highly distractible retarded children in increasing their attending behavior in a training and two generalization (a one-to-one and a classroom) situations. Three untrained sub- jects were monitored for general comparison and^ social validation^ purposes. One^ of these control subjects was distractible and the other two (criterion comparison) were evaluated as not having attentional problems. A multiple baseline design was employed in which training was sequentially introduced across subjects. During training, the ex- perimental subjects were taught through self-instruction to focus their attention and to cope with two tasks, math and printing. After learning the self-instructions the subjects were systematically and sequentially exposed to photo-slides of distracting situations, to audio-distractors composed of noisy lunchroom^ verbal^ peer interactions, and^ to^ in vivo distractors provided by kindergarten children^ playing with^ wooden^ blocks^ in the^ training setting. The entire training procedure was handled in^ a^ game-like context to^ maintain subject interest and to facilitate generalization. The results suggested that the training package produced direct and generalized changes in^ self-instructional behavior. In addi- tion, a decrease in off-task behavior occurred during math, printing, and also during a phonics program in the one-to-one and classroom situations. However, reliable changes in academic task performance were not observed. Finally, no systematic changes on^ any of the dependent measures occurred for the three untrained subjects. DESCRIPTORS: self-instruction, attending behavior, academic behavior, generaliza- tion, retarded children

Within educational settings, teachers recog- nize that students must sustain attention to school-related materials and activities if learning

This paper is based on a thesis completed by the first author^ in^ partial fulfillment^ of^ the^ requirements for the master of arts (^) degree in (^) psychology at the University of^ Notre^ Dame.^ The^ research^ reported herein was supported by a National Research Train- ing Award #HD07184-01. Sections of this paper were presented at the annual convention of the Asso- ciation for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, San Francisco, December, 1979. The authors thank Carol (^) Glass, Kathy Larsen, John Scibak, Sandy Levers, and Jim Little for their assistance in the project. Spe- cial thanks go to John Borkowski, Ellen Ryan, Denny Reid, and Tom Merluzzi for their helpful comments on (^) the manuscript. Thanks are also due to Sr. Ann Fox and the children of St. Bavos elementary school for their cooperation throughout the project. Reprints are available from Lou Burgio or Tom Whitman, De- partment of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana 46556.

is to occur. Where attentional deficits exist, ini- tial emphasis is often placed on the teacher for

ameliorating this problem. Since the advent of

behavior modification a variety of techniques have been developed to modify distractible, non- attentive behaviors in normal and retarded popu- lations, including: positive reinforcement of in- compatible behaviors (Alabiso, 1975; Patterson, 1965; Patterson, Jones, Whittier, & Wright, 1965; Whitman, Caponigri, & Mercurio, 1971), time out procedures (Johnson, Whitman, & Bar- toon-Noble, 1978), and aversive stimulation (Forehand & Baumeister, 1970; Reardon & Bell, 1970). Recently, a number of behavioral therapists have emphasized the need to switch the locus of control for an individual's behavior from exter- nal agents (e.g., the teacher) to the individual himself (cf. Kazdin, 1975). They argue that 443

1980, 139 443-459^ NUMBER^3 (FALL 1980)

LOUIS D. BURGIO et (^) al.

teaching individuals to control their own behav- ior is educationally more efficient in that it frees the teacher from many (^) routine supervision re- sponsibilities and because once learned it allows children to effect positive changes more readily in their own behavior across situations. One ap- proach to teaching individuals to control their own (^) behavior has involved self-instructional training. The self-instructional approach evolved from early investigations of the effects of verbal

operants on motoric behavior (Bem, 1967; Lo-

vaas, 1964; Luria, 1961; Vygotsky, 1962). The results of these studies suggest that verbal in- structional procedures can facilitate the acquisi- tion (^) of new behaviors and increase appropriate responding in^ a^ variety of^ situations. Since these seminal investigations, self-in- structional procedures have been used success- fully in modifying a wide range of behaviors

(cf. Burron & Bucker, 1978; Meichenbaum,

1975), although researchers have confronted some difficulty in effecting change in academic

task behavior (cf. Robin, Armel, & O'Leary,

1975). With the exception of a study by Gural- nick (1976), research has typically employed adults (^) and children who (^) are of normal intelli- gence. Guralnick (1976) compared the effective- ness of feedback, modeling, and self-instruction techniques in developing problem-solving strate- gies for complex perceptual discriminations with a group of educable mentally retarded children. Results indicate that only the self-instructional approach significantly increases performance. In the present study the (^) utility of a self-in- structional program with highly distractible re- tarded children was examined. The use of a self- instructional procedure with hyperactive and highly distractible children was suggested by Luria (^) (1961). He stated that incorporating the child's own speech in a treatment program for hyperactivity would decrease the opportunity for disruption of goal-directed behavior and fa- cilitate the organization of the children's own activities. Guralnick (1976) has similarly con- tended that self-instructions channel an individ-

ual's attention skills in selecting the relevant

cues in a situation. In two earlier studies (Palkes,

Stewart, & Freedman, 1972; Palkes, Stewart, &

Kahana, 1968) the authors found that training

hyperactive children to use self-directed verbal commands improved posttest performance on

the Porteus Maze (a measure of impulsivity)

when compared with control subjects who sim- ply practiced the training exercises. However, until the present study no attempt to evaluate the effects of self-instructional training on dis- tractible retarded children has been conducted. The self-instructional (^) package used in (^) this study provided to the subjects: strategic inocula- tion and coping self-statements to assist them, respectively, in completing arithmetic and print- ing assignments, in^ ignoring distracting stimuli, and in dealing with task failure. In addition, be- cause behavior modification programs have not always been successful in^ effecting generalized behavior changes across situations (Stokes & Baer, 1977), several procedures for facilitating generalization into a one-to-one and classroom setting were introduced. Specifically, during

training a "storylike" instructional procedure

(Bornstein & Quevillon, 1976) employing class- room imagery was used to assist the children in

recognizing situations where self-instruction was

appropriate; distracting stimuli similar to those likely to be encountered in the classroom were

introduced; and finally, multiple exemplars

(Stokes & Baer, 1977) of academic tasks encoun-

tered in the classroom were employed. In order

to evaluate the direct and generalized effects of training, the subjects' use of self-instruction in the training situation, in a one-to-one and in a classroom situation, was assessed. In addition, changes in the subjects' (^) off-task behavior in the latter two generalization situations were moni- tored. The final goal of this study was to examine

whether correlated changes in academic behavior

occurred. In order to evaluate this question, per- manent-product data were collected during the math, printing, and phonics programs in the generalization settings.

444

LOUIS D. BURGIO et al.

events which (^) commonly occurred in the class- room (transfer II) setting.

Response (^) Definitions and Rating System

Self-instructional verbalizations were rated to assess whether the training program succeeded in teaching children to self-instruct and whether the children self-instructed in the transfer situa- tions. Off-task behavior was assessed in the trans- fer situations to see if there would be a change in the frequency of this response during self-in- structional training. These target behaviors were

defined as follows:

Self-instructional verbalization-Statements

made by the child pertaining to the appropriate

performance of a task. Specifically, a child was

rated as self-instructing when he or she made one of the (^) following statements: (1) asked a question [e.g., (^) "What does Sr. _ (the

teacher) want me to do?") (2) answered the

question (e.g., "She wants me to draw this word."); (3) provided direction on how to to do

the task (e.g., "First, I should look at both num-

bers," "I take away 4."); (4) reinforced himself

or (^) herself for completing the task (e.g., "I did

a good job," "I'm doing real good so far."); (5)

provided a cue to ignore distraction (e.g., "I hear

people talking but I'm not going to let them

bother me."); (6) specified how to cope with

task-failure (^) (e.g., "Oh, I was (^) messy in (^) printing

that word. That's okay, I'll^ be even more careful

on the next word."). In-seat, off-task-The child is sitting in his or her chair with buttocks touching the seat of the chair but is not performing the assigned task properly. For example, the student might be looking about the classroom or (^) talking to a class- mate but would not be (^) looking at the task material. Ratings were taken for each experimental stu-

dent three times a week on Monday, Wednesday,

and Friday mornings in the training and two transfer (^) settings. A fourth afternoon (^) rating ses- sion, scheduled randomly, was also^ taken^ on one of these (^) days each week. (^) During each session

behaviors were recorded during a 15-min train-

ing period, a 5-min transfer I period, and during a 1 5-min transfer II (classroom) period while the students performed the arithmetic, printing, and phonics (classroom only) tasks. In the training and transfer I, but not in the transfer II (classroom) settings, the six types of self-instruction (e.g., "question," "answer") were subcoded in an event fashion. Specific self-in- structional components were not recorded in the classroom because the noise level in the room made it difficult to (^) obtain a (^) reliable measure (^) of these behaviors in this setting. In the transfer I and II settings the incidence of self-instruction, irrespective of type, was rated in an interval fashion. An interval recording system was also used to rate off-task behavior in the transfer I. and transfer II (^) settings. Off-task behavior was not recorded in the training setting. When (^) rating self-instruction and off-task behavior with an in- terval system, these responses were scored in terms of their occurrence or non-occurrence within successive 10-sec intervals. Each specific behavior was recorded only once within an in- terval. A cassette tape recorder was used to record

self-instructions in the^ training setting. A^ video-

tape recorder was used to record off-task and

self-instructional behaviors in the transfer I pe-

riod and off-task behavior in the classroom set- ting. Thus, all ratings of the target responses

were made from these tapes except for self-in-

structional behavior in the classroom which was rated as it occurred. Performance measures-In addition to the preceding behavioral measures, measures of the

subjects' performance quality on the arithmetic,

printing, and phonics tasks were taken daily in the 15-min classroom situation. The overall

number of problems completed on the arithme-

tic task and the number of letters printed on the printing task were tallied for each session. In addition, a percent correct measure was calcu- lated for each task by counting the number of

correct answers given or letters printed and di-

viding them by the total number of problems completed or letters printed. The writing task

SELF-INSTRUCTIONAL PACKAGE FOR ATTENDING BEHAVIOR

was evaluated by a method derived from Hel- wig, Johns, Norman, and Cooper (1976) which utilized transparent overlays (^) to measure devia- tions of writing samples. A deviation of more than 2 mm from the standard was considered an incorrect response.

Children's distractibility rating form-In or-

der to assess the teacher's perceptions of how distractible the children in this study were, a dis- tractibility rating form composed of five ques- tions was administered to her prior to the initia- tion of the study and again at the conclusion of training. The questions on this form probed the teacher's views concerning the children's (^) ability to (^) maintain attention to their work (e.g., "Does this child (^) have trouble concentrating and keep- ing his mind on one thing?") and the extent of teacher prompting needed to assist the child in completing a task (e.g., "Does this child require prompting to finish his work?"). A 5-point scale indicating the extent to which the child displayed a particular (^) problem was used by the teacher in answering the questions.

Reliability Assessment For reliability purposes, at least twice in each condition a second observer rated the students' behaviors from randomly selected videotapes of the transfer situations and cassette recordings of the training sessions. Similarly, for the same pur- pose, a second observer was brought into the transfer II (^) (classroom) setting to assess self-in- structional (^) behavior. In calculating reliability coefficients for off-task behavioral and global self-instructional ratings in the transfer I and transfer II^ settings, individual rating records were compared on an interval-by-interval basis for each child. The reliability coefficients (^) were calculated by dividing the number of agreements of occurrence of the behaviors between the two observers by the total number of observations. For off-task (^) behavior, interobserver agreement

ranged from 53 to 100% with a mean of 83%.

Interobserver agreement for global self-instruc-

tion ranged from 73 to 100% with a mean (^) of

87%. All of the low reliability coefficients (<80%) obtained, occurred when the fre- quency of off-task and global self-instruction was extremely low (<4 occurrences). This occurred

three times. The reliability of the event recording

system used in measuring the occurrence of specific component self-instructions was assessed through the following formula: % agreement = (^) smaller number of occurrence/larger number of occurrence (^) X 100. The (^) percentage agreement across all reliability checks^ ranged from 81 to 100% with a mean of (^) 90%. That (^) is, in no in- stance did observer agreement, when a specific self-instructional component was rated, fall be- low 80%.

In order to assess the reliability of the rating

system used in evaluating the children's perfor- mance on the academic (^) tasks, each child's task performance was rated at least twice during each phase by dividing the number of agreements of

occurrence by the total number of agreements

and disagreements. Specific reliability coefficients

for the accuracy measure ranged across children

and tasks, from 97 to 100% with a mean of 99 %. Interobserver agreement for the rate mea- sure was (^) 100% at all times.

Design A multiple baseline design across children was employed. Training procedures were sequen- tially introduced to the two experimental chil- dren in the training setting. No intervention oc- curred in the transfer I or classroom setting. The behavior of the control and criterion comparison children, who did not receive (^) training, was moni- tored (^) throughout the (^) study in the (^) classroom.

Procedure Baseline. (^) For the two experimental children and one control (^) child, behavioral ratings were

taken in the training, transfer I, and classroom

situations. The two criterion comparison chil-

dren were observed only in the classroom setting.

No experimental manipulations were initiated

447

SELF-INSTRUCTIONAL PACKAGE FOR ATTENDING BEHAVIOR

another child who is trying to complete a task). The audio distractors were supplied via a tape recording of the children's peers interacting loudly in a lunchroom setting. Finally, to supply in vivo distraction, kindergarten children were introduced into the training setting and in- structed to play with wooden blocks and other noise-inducing (^) objects. During the initial presentations of the various distractors, the experimenter modeled the distrac- tion-ignoring self-statement by saying: "If I were in the classroom doing my work and saying the words and something like this happened (i.e., the visual, audio, or in vivo distraction), I would say something like this: 'I'm not gonna look, I'm (^) gonna keep doing my work.'" As each dis- tracting stimulus was (^) introduced the child was asked to identify it (e.g., "People are making noise."), and then asked to verbalize the distrac- tion-ignoring self-statement. When the subject could verbalize this appropriate self-statement along with the previously learned self-statements for a given set of distractors (^) (visual or audio) unprompted for three consecutive sessions, the next (^) type of distractors was introduced. The chil-

dren were^ continued^ in the in^ vivo^ distraction phase until training was terminated.

Maintenance Due to time limitations caused by the end of the academic year, only limited data were col- lected in this condition and only for one child. The first child was (^) gradually (over a 4-wk (^) pe- riod) (^) given fewer training sessions, at a rate of one less per week, until the last week of the study at which time training was phased out com- pletely.

RESULTS

Self-Instructional Behavior Training setting. Table 1 shows the mean unprompted frequencies of the various self-in- structional components during the training con- ditions. Although no self-instructions occurred prior to training, after training both experimen- tal children learned to verbalize each of the self- instructional (^) components without (^) prompting. The low mean frequencies of occurrence of the coping self-instruction in the math and printing

Table 1 Mean frequency of various types of self-instruction by the experimental children across tasks (math and printing) during the training condition in the training and transfer I settings.,

Self- Children Question Answer Task Coping Distraction reinforcement

TRAINING Math Judy 12.7 1.0 59.3 1.0 8.7 14. Angie 15.6 1.3 65.3 0.0 11.3 13. Printing Judy 1.6^ 1.2^ 161.4 4.1 14.3 14. Angie 2.5 1.4 199.1 0.3 11.4 14.

TRANSFER I Math Judy 8.4^ 0.9 32.1 0.3 0.0 9. Angie 5.4^ 0.7 33.2 0.0 0.2 4. Printing Judy 1.1 1.0 118.6 0.8 0.1 9. Angie 0.9 0.5 83.6 0.0 0.1 4. aSelf-instruction did not occur during the baseline condition.

449

LOUIS D. BURGIO et a.

tasks and the question self-instruction in the printing task were a function of the nature of the rating system and differential task demands. Although not presented in Table 1, the data also show that both children began producing some unprompted self-instructions as early as the first training session. Transfer I setting. (^) Figure 1 shows the per- centage of intervals in which general self-in- structional behavior (collapsing across the vari- ous types of self-instructions) occurred across tasks and conditions for the experimental chil- dren and for the (^) untrained child in (^) the transfer I setting. The two (^) children who received training showed a high frequency of self-instruction on both tasks in this generalization situation whereas neither of these children prior to train- ing nor the third child who never received train-

ing showed any self-instructional behavior. Both

of the trained children displayed a higher fre- quency of self-instruction on the printing task. Table 1 shows the mean frequencies of the vari- ous self-instructional components that were emitted in the transfer I setting by the two ex- perimental children on the math (^) and printing tasks. With the exception of the (^) coping and dis-

traction self-instructions, the other self-instruc-

tional components occurred on the average of

at least once and usually much more frequently

each session. The lower frequency of distraction-

ignoring self-instructions can be attributed to the lower number of distractors available in the transfer I situation. Transfer II (classroom) setting. Figure 2 shows the frequency of self-instruction by the experimental children and the untrained chil- dren during the math, printing, and phonics tasks in the classroom (^) setting. Although both experimental children showed self-instructional behavior in this generalization setting, they were quite variable. As Figure 2 shows, Angie dis- played a higher rate of self-instruction than Judy. Both children self-instructed more fre- quently during the printing than the math task.

With the exception of one of Angie's sessions,

neither (^) child showed self-instruction during the

phonics task. The third child who was not trained did not self-instruct nor did the criterion reference children whose data are not presented.

Off-Task Transfer I and II (classroom) settings. (^) Figures 3 and 4 show the frequency of off-task behavior displayed by the two experimental children and the untrained child in the transfer I and transfer II (classroom) setting. Data for the criterion comparison children are also presented in the classroom situation. Figure 3 shows that off-task behavior was (^) generally low across conditions (^) for all three (^) children in the transfer (^) I setting, al- though the data suggest that Angie was less off task after she received self-instructional training. The low rate of off-task behaviors in this situa- tion is probably related to the fact that there were no external distractors in the room. In (^) contrast, Figure 4 shows that (^) off-task be- havior was generally higher in (^) the transfer II (classroom) setting than in the transfer I setting. The two experimental children displayed a grad- ual but marked decrease in off-task behavior after training. Generally, this effect occurred across all tasks. Due to the nature and complex- ity of data presented in Figure 4, for these two

children a time-series statistical analysis (Glass,

Willson, & Gottman, 1974) was^ performed as

an adjunct to visual analysis.1 For Angie, the

time-series analysis showed a significant change

in both trend, t(41) =^ 5.37, p < 0.0005, one-

tailed, and level, t(41) =^ 1.72, p < 0.05, one-

tailed, of her off-task behavior during the math

task, a significant change in trend, t(41) -

1.97, p < 0.05, (^) one-tailed, during the printing

task, and a significant change in^ trend, t(41)

7.07, p < 0.0005, one-tailed, during the^ phonics

task. For Judy there was a significant post-inter-

vention change in the level of her off-task be-

havior during the printing, t(40) =^ 1.61, p <

'The authors performed the time-series analyses with the TMS (^) computer program developed by Cath- leen Bower, William Padia, and Gene Glass at the Laboratory of^ Educational Research, University of Colorado, October^ 1974.

LOUIS D. BURGIO et al.

BASELINE TREATMENT

HARVEY

0 6 12 la 24 30 37 43

SESSIONS Fig. 2. Percentage of intervals of self-instruction over sessions by the experimental children on the math, printing, and phonics tasks in the transfer II (classroom) setting. (An asterisk designates a 2-week^ school holiday when observations were not made. The arrow signifies the point at which^ training was faded out^ for Judy.)

452

0 Ca 0

(^0) CoCD

0

10

0

-J

LO z

LLU L)J CL

(^00)

(^0) Go

(^0) CD

0

0

0

(^00)

(^0) Co

0

0

(^0) ...........................&...

SELF-INSTRUCTIONAL PACKAGE FOR ATTENDING BEHAVIOR

BASEOLNE TREATMENT

JUDY

iAA X2 $

0 6 12 18 24 30 37 43

jw~

  • -@ MATH &- la^ PRINTING

ANRGIE

HARVEY

(^0 6 12) 2a

'as

30 37 1aS

SESSIONS Fig. (^) 3. Percentage of (^) intervals of off-task behavior over sessions by the experimental children on the math and (^) printing tasks in the (^) transfer I (^) setting. (An asterisk designates a 2-week school holiday when observations were not (^) made. The arrow (^) signifies the point at which training was faded out for Judy.)

a

a"

aco

ax

no a

x

(n -J CR

C: LJ I-"z z

LU

cc

Gi

a co

co 'V

a

0

co as

a

Cy

a (^) mmm&.

SELF-INSTRUCTIONAL PACKAGE FOR ATTENDING BEHAVIOR

Table 2 Mean rate and accuracy of performance during math, printing, and (^) phonics tasks (^) for experimental and criterion comparison children (^) during baseline and (^) training conditions in the transfer II (classroom) setting.ab

Accuracy Rate Children Baseline Training (^) Baseline Training Math Judy (^) 71.6(41.5) 89.0(14.4) 4.0(2.1) (^) 13.1(6.2) Angie (^) 42.6(22.4) 56.3(16.9) 7.3(3.1) (^) 6.6(1.5) Donny 91.4(10.1) 87.3(13.8) (^) 10.2(5.3) 13.1(4.6) Kathy 47.7(28.4) (^) 35.2(25.2) 4.8(1.4) 4.0(1.6) Printing Judy (^) 92.6(3.4) 93.6(6.6) 43.8(12.4) (^) 43.5(13.2) Angie (^) 66.5(16.3) 67.8(11.3) 34.5(9.9) 26.4(5.7) Donny (^) 91.6(5.6) 91.5(10.1) 74.0(35.2) (^) 51.1(13.3) Kathy 87.7(6.8) 86.0(8.5) (^) 33.9(5.7) 30.6(4.9) Phonicsc Judy 82.4(18.7) (^) 83.5(18.7) Angie 53.3(33.7) 69.7(14.8) Donny 88.9(18.8) (^) 82.6(27.6) Kathy 67.6(29.4) (^) 80.0(24.5) aThe standard deviations appear in parentheses. bMeans and standard deviations for criterion (^) comparison children (Donny and Kathy) during baseline and training conditions were computed on (^) the task data for the first and last 23 sessions, (^) respectively. cRate data are not reported for (^) the phonics task due to the variable number of phonics (^) problems given in any one session.

Children's Distractibility Rating Form admin-

istered before and after training were compared.

The scale scores ranged from 0 to 4, with 4 in-

dicating high activity and 0 indicating a low

level of activity. Both experimental (^) children who received training were rated as (^) being less "hy- peractive" after (^) training than before training. Judy's and (^) Angie's prebaseline activity ratings were at a (^3) level, whereas their ratings after training were, respectively, 1.2 and 2.2. Both criterion reference children (Donny and Kathy) were rated as being slightly more active at the end of the study (1.6 and 1.4) than (^) before the study began (1.0 and 0.6) and were similar at this (^) point in their activity level to the experi- mental children. (^) At the end of the study the in- dividual seen by the (^) teacher as least "hyperac- tive" was Judy, an experimental (^) child.

DISCUSSION

From both (^) an experimental and clinical per- spective the (^) results of this study suggest that the

self-instructional training program employed can be used to decrease the off-task behavior of dis-

tractible retarded children in a classroom setting.

In contrast to most previous (^) research that has

used this approach, the present study not only

trained children to self-instruct but systemati-

cally monitored^ the extent to which the children

learned these self-instructions during training

and subsequently exhibited them in a nontrain- ing situation. The results indicated that the (^) ex- perimental children learned to (^) self-instruct in the training situation and to use these (^) self-in- structions extensively for the math and (^) printing tasks in the transfer I (^) situation where only an observer and the child were present. In (^) both the training and the transfer I situation it was evi- dent not only that both experimental children were self-instructing frequently (^) but also that they were using all of the (^) self-instructional com- ponents they had been taught. The results also indicate that both children self-instructed, albeit to a lesser extent, in the classroom situation. However, it may be that

455

LOUIS D. (^) BURGIO et (^) al.

more self-instructions occurred than were rated in this latter setting. Both children, in the early stages of treatment, spontaneously reported that they said (^) the words (self-instructions) to them- selves while they were doing their work in the classroom. Moreover, while the observer was re-

cording self-instructions in the classroom setting,

the children were often noticed mouthing, in- audibly, (^) what appeared to be (^) self-instructions. However, due to the absence of clear and definite identification, these apparent self-instructions were not recorded formally. Perhaps in future research a wireless microphone could detect these low volume verbalizations. In general, however, the fact that less self-instruction oc- curred in the classroom is consistent with past research. Meichenbaum (1977) has suggested that the (^) classroom setting may indeed have an inhibitory effect on overt self-instruction. One can easily understand how children may be re- luctant to self-instruct overtly in the presence of their peers for fear of drawing adverse attention to themselves. The self-instructional program used in (^) this study (^) differed in (^) several significant respects from those employed in past studies. First, it was more complex in that it taught the children self-in- structions for two very different tasks as well as procedures to cope with error and distraction, included within a game-like context. Second, the distraction (^) inoculation procedure was quite unique in that it not only taught the children what they should say when (^) distraction occurred, but (^) also, systematically, through the presenta- tions of a variety of visual, audio, and in vivo stimuli, showed the children the occasions when such statements would be appropriate. Third, because in pilot research with retarded children, modeling and prompting were not found suffi- cient to (^) produce good self-instructional behavior, the experimenter shaped, with praise as a (^) rein-

forcer, the chain of the various self-instructional

components to^ be used by the children. Although it^ took several sessions for the chil- dren to learn to self-instruct (^) without prompting, both children appeared to be (^) enjoying the train-

ing sessions and both reported that they enjoyed

playing the "game." One child even reported

that she played the "game" at home while doing her homework. These anecdotal data support the contention that by presenting the self-instruc- tional training as a "game that will (^) help you do your work better," children's attention will be better maintained during training (Meichen- baum, 1977; Bash & Camp, Note 1). This pro- cedure may be especially important with a re- tarded population that has marked attentional problems (House & Zeaman, 1963). As indicated in (^) Figure 4, there were marked decreases in the off-task behavior of the two ex- perimental (^) children in the classroom setting. Moreover, there is an obvious, although imper- fect, correlation between the self-instructional behavior of these children and their off-task be- havior during both the math and printing tasks (See Figures 2 and 4). (^) Conversely, the untrained child and the criterion comparison children (^) who did (^) not receive training did not show changes in either their (^) self-instructional or off-task be- havior. The direct monitoring of both of these subject behaviors allows stronger inferences to be made regarding whether the changes in off- task behavior were actually a function of the self-instructional program (Kazdin, (^) 1978). Al-

though similar reductions in off-task behavior

have been found by Meichenbaum and (^) Good- man (^) (1971), Bornstein and Quevillon (1976), and others, they did not monitor self-instruction directly. The results of this study suggest that the self- instructional (^) training package employed effected generalized changes in self-instructional and off- task behavior across situations, most importantly in the classroom, and across tasks. Prior to this

study only Bornstein and Quevillon (1976),

using a^ self-instructional program, obtained gen- eralization of behavioral (^) effects into a new set- ting, but (^) with a nonretarded population. One aspect of^ the^ results might, however, argue against the (^) conclusion that the self-instructional behavior and the (^) generalized effects (^) were really functionally related. That is, although decreases

458 LOUIS D. BURGIO et al.

common way of making this assessment is to ask "significant individuals" who are affected in im- portant ways by the child's behavior to assess whether the child is different after training. The problem with this type of assessment is that it often does not have established reliability and validity (O'Leary & Turkewitz, 1978). In the present research several methods of assessing the clinical significance of the decrease in the experi- mental children's off-task behavior were in- cluded. The teacher's perceptions of the chil-

dren's behavior, before and after training, were

solicited. In addition, the experimental children's behavior throughout the study was compared with that of two criterion comparison children who were seen as good students (frequently on

task) by the teacher. Both the teacher's ratings

and the interchild comparisons suggested that

after training the experimental children approxi-

mated and in^ some cases were off-task less than the criterion children in the classroom. The child who was most distractible in the classroom dur- ing baseline appeared after training more task oriented than the criterion comparison children. In summary, the results of this study indicate

that the self-instructional training package pro-

duced direct changes in^ self-instructional behav- ior in the training situation and that generalized change occurred in the self-instructional behav- ior of the experimental children in the transfer I and II (classroom) situations, and in their off- task behavior during three task situations in the classroom. It was established that retarded chil-

dren can^ be^ taught self-instructions^ even^ though

they are language deficient and commonly thought unable to gain control over their own behavior. Furthermore, this study suggests that self-instruction might be used to^ reduce hyper-

activity, a common^ problem among retarded

children. The results of this study are generally encouraging, but they do suggest that when a self-instructional approach is to be used to effect changes in academic performance, the instruc- tions employed may well need to be modified. In this regard and more generally, it is impera- tive that the^ self-instructional technology not be

viewed as a completed product, to be used but not refined. Although this approach should be compared ultimately with more direct behavior change procedures, the authors would argue that the self-instructional approach is still in its be- ginning stages of development, and that compar- isons of self-instruction with other techniques, such as token systems (cf. Friedling & O'Leary, 1979), is premature.

REFERENCE NOTES

  1. Bash, M., & Camp, B. Think aloud (^) program: Group manual.^ Unpublished^ manuscript.^ Univer- sity of Colorado Medical School, 1975.
  2. Wein, K. S., & Nelson, R. 0.^ The^ effects^ of^ self- instructional training on^ arithmetic^ problem^ solv- ing skills. Unpublished manuscript.^ University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 1978.

REFERENCES Alabiso, F. Operant control of attentive behavior: A treatment for hyperactivity. Behavior Therapy, 1975, 6, 39-42. Bem, S.^ L.^ Verbal^ self-control:^ The^ establishment of (^) effective self-instruction. Journal of Experi- mental (^) Psychology, 1967, 74, 4, 485-491. Bornstein, P. H., & Quevillon, R.^ P.^ The^ effects^ of a self-instructional package on overactive pre- school boys. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal- ysis, 1976, 9, 179-188. Burron, D., & Bucher, B. Self-instructions as dis- criminative cues for rule breaking or rule follow- ing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1978, 26, 46-57. Ferritor, D.^ E.,^ Buckholdt,^ D.,^ Hamblin,^ R.^ C.,^ & Smith, L.^ The^ noneffects^ of^ contingent^ rein- forcement for attending behavior on work accom- plished. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1972, 5, 7-17. Forehand, R., & Baumeister, A. A. Effects of^ varia- tion in^ auditory-visual stimulation on^ activity lev- els of^ severe mental^ retardates.^ American^ Journal of Mental^ Deficiency,^ 1970,^ 74,^ 470-474. Frielding, C., & O'Leary, S. G. Effects of self-in- structional training on second- and third-grade hyperactive children: A failure to replicate. Jour- nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1979, 12, 21 1-

Glass, G.^ V., Willson,^ V.^ L.,^ &^ Gottman, J. M.^ De- sign and Analysis of Time-series experiment. Boulder: Colorado Associated University Press,

SELF-INSTRUCTIONAL PACKAGE FOR ATTENDING BEHAVIOR 459

Guralnick, M. J. Solving complex perceptual dis- crimination problems: (^) Techniques for the (^) devel- opment of (^) problem-solving strategies. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1976, 81, 1, 118-

Helwig, J. J., Johns, J. C., Normal, J. E., and Cooper, J. O. The measurement of manuscript letter strokes. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1976, 9, 231-236. House, B. J., & Zeaman, D. The role of attention in retardate discrimination learning. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), Handbook of mental (^) deficiency: Psycho- logical theory and research. New York: (^) McGraw- Hill, 1963. Johnson, M. R., (^) Whitman, T. (^) L., & Bartoon-Noble, R. A (^) home-based program for a preschool behavior- ally disturbed child with parents as therapists. Journal of Behavior (^) Therapy and (^) Experimental Psychiatry, 1978, 9, 65-70. Kazdin, A. E. Behavior modification in applied set- tings. Homewood, Ill.: The Dorsey Press, 1975. Kazdin, A. E. Methodological and interpretive prob- lems of single-case experimental designs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1978, 46, 629-642. Lovaas, 0. I. Cue properties of (^) words: The control of operant responding by (^) rate and content of ver- bal operants. Child (^) Development, 1964, 35, 245-

Luria, A. R. The (^) role of speech in the regulation of normal and (^) abnormal behavior. New York: Live- right, 1961. Meichenbaum, D. H. Enhancing creativity by mod- ifying what subjects say to themselves. American Educational Research Journal, 1975, 12, 129-145. Meichenbaum, D. H. Cognitive-behavior Modifica- tion: (^) An integrative approach. New York: Plenum Press, 1977. Meichenbaum, D. H., & Goodman, J. Training im- pulsive children to talk to (^) themselves: A means of developing self-control. Journal (^) of Abnormal Psychology, 1971, 77, (^) 115-126. O'Leary, K.^ D., Becker, W. C., Evans, M. B., & Sau- dergas, R.^ A. A token reinforcement program in a (^) public school: A replication and systematic anal-

ysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1969, 2, 3-13. O'Leary, K.^ D., &^ Turkewitz, H.^ Methodological errors in marital and child treatment research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1978, 46, 4, 747-758. Palkes, H., Stewart, M., & Freedman, J. Improve- ment in maze performance of hyperactive boys as a function of verbal-training procedures. The Journal of Special Education, 1972, 5, 4, 337-

Palkes, H., Stewart, M., & Kahana, B. Porteus maze performance of^ hyperactive boys after^ training in self-directed verbal commands. Child^ Develop- ment, 1968,39, 817-826. Patterson, G. R. An application of conditioning techniques to the control of a hyperactive child. In L. P. Ullman and L. Krasner (Eds.), Case stud- ies in behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. Patterson, G. R., Jones, R., Whittier, J., & Wright, M. A. A behavior modification (^) technique for the (^) hyperactive child. Behavior (^) Research and Therapy, 1965, 2, 217-226. Reardon, D.^ M., & Bell, G. Effects of sedative and stimulative music on activity levels of severely re- tarded boys. American Journal of Mental Defi- ciency, 1970, 75, 156-159. Robin, A. L., Armel, S., & O'Leary, K. D. The ef- fects of self-instruction on writing deficiencies. Behavior Therapy, 1975, 6, 178-187. Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D.^ M.^ An implicit technology of (^) generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1977, 10, 349-367. Surratt, P. R., Ulrich, P. E., & Hawkins, R. P. An elementary student as a behavioral engineer. Jour- nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1969, 2, 85-92. Vygotsky, L. Thought and language. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1962. Whitman, T. L., Caponigri, V., & Mercurio, J. Re- ducing hyperactive behavior in a severely retarded child. Mental Retardation, 1971, 9, (^) 17-19.

Received July 23, 1979 Final acceptance March 13, 1980