Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Gordon Allport’s Trait Theory, Study notes of Personality Psychology

The dynamic substructures of which a personality is composed are unique integrations formed in the individual course of experience and heredity.

Typology: Study notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 03/31/2022

aristel
aristel 🇺🇸

4.2

(34)

316 documents

1 / 5

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
Gordon Allport’s Trait Theory
In attempting to formulate an account of personality, Allport rejected the notion that what
constitutes personality can in any way be traced back to, or attributed to, innate physiological
processes (as McCrae and Costa have been doing). As he argued, as Leont’ev (1978) would
later, newborns lack a personality (and the personality traits which have yet to form).
Only the rudiments of that “which is highest and most excellent in man” are given at
birth. The fully fashioned social and moral being, the developed adult personality,
waits upon the process of growth. The nature of growth is the critical problem for the
psychology of personality. For above all else it must know how the biological
organism it finds at birth becomes transformed into the adult person able to take his
place in the highly complex social activities of the civilized world surrounding him.
(Allport, 1937, p. 101)
The psychology of personality has to be a post-instinctive formation.
According to Allport, “the course of individuality is one of greater and greater divergence
from the relatively standard pattern of infancy. The dynamic substructures of which a
personality is composed are unique integrations formed in the individual course of experience
and heredity” (1937, p. 245). Whatever innate conditions may be present at birth, over the
course of development and subsequent experience, they are transformed by learning; their
motivational force is recast, possibly inhibited or transformed. The fetish of the genetic
method which searches for the origin of the present in the past requires elimination. The adult
is functionally independent of the infant. In the course of adaptation to current conditions the
individual becomes socialized and civilized, and the group’s standards are introcepted. The
outer (possibly alien) is transformed into the inner (which is consistent with Vygotsky’s 1981
general genetic method of cultural development). This meant that “since personality is largely
a matter of the introception and modification of social conventions, customs and codes, it is
instructive to know to what cultural stimuli and models the individual is exposed in the
course of his development” (Allport, 1937, p. 371). This applied equally to personality traits.
In attempting to identify a unit of analysis in the assessment of personality, Allport, as was
just noted, rejected any kind of natural endowment as being in any way adequate or
sufficient. From the start these innate endowments or temperament were subject to the forces
of learning or classical conditioning and were, in the processes, transformed. A reflexive
response is transferred to a neutral stimulus. Over subsequent learning experiences the same
and similar conditioned responses form into habits, or systems of integrated conditioned
responses, which are stereotyped responses that are called up under similar recurring
situations. To be serviceable for survival, in Allport’s judgment, the personality unit had to be
stable and flexible. To survive it was necessary to learn to behave in response to varied
stimuli that are perceived as similar and to adapt to novel conditions. Such demands could not
be met by habits. Habits were an inflexible type of response that were evoked by a narrow
range of stimulus conditions, in particular situations.
In the hustle and bustle of the human realm of active engagement, particularly in highly
complex sociocultural environments, incapacity to respond readily to novelty would not be
functionally useful. Malleability was called for. Allport’s preferred unit, therefore, was based
on the innate endowment being transformed into habits by conditioning, and, subsequently,
the fusion of habits, having the same adaptive significance, into a higher-order system of
organization—traits. Once formed, traits were functionally independent of their composite
pf3
pf4
pf5

Partial preview of the text

Download Gordon Allport’s Trait Theory and more Study notes Personality Psychology in PDF only on Docsity!

Gordon Allport’s Trait Theory

In attempting to formulate an account of personality, Allport rejected the notion that what constitutes personality can in any way be traced back to, or attributed to, innate physiological processes (as McCrae and Costa have been doing). As he argued, as Leont’ev (1978) would later, newborns lack a personality (and the personality traits which have yet to form).

Only the rudiments of that “which is highest and most excellent in man” are given at birth. The fully fashioned social and moral being, the developed adult personality, waits upon the process of growth. The nature of growth is the critical problem for the psychology of personality. For above all else it must know how the biological organism it finds at birth becomes transformed into the adult person able to take his place in the highly complex social activities of the civilized world surrounding him. (Allport, 1937, p. 101)

The psychology of personality has to be a post-instinctive formation.

According to Allport, “the course of individuality is one of greater and greater divergence from the relatively standard pattern of infancy. The dynamic substructures of which a personality is composed are unique integrations formed in the individual course of experience and heredity” (1937, p. 245). Whatever innate conditions may be present at birth, over the course of development and subsequent experience, they are transformed by learning; their motivational force is recast, possibly inhibited or transformed. The fetish of the genetic method which searches for the origin of the present in the past requires elimination. The adult is functionally independent of the infant. In the course of adaptation to current conditions the individual becomes socialized and civilized, and the group’s standards are introcepted. The outer (possibly alien) is transformed into the inner (which is consistent with Vygotsky’s 1981 general genetic method of cultural development ). This meant that “since personality is largely a matter of the introception and modification of social conventions, customs and codes, it is instructive to know to what cultural stimuli and models the individual is exposed in the course of his development” (Allport, 1937, p. 371). This applied equally to personality traits.

In attempting to identify a unit of analysis in the assessment of personality, Allport, as was just noted, rejected any kind of natural endowment as being in any way adequate or sufficient. From the start these innate endowments or temperament were subject to the forces of learning or classical conditioning and were, in the processes, transformed. A reflexive response is transferred to a neutral stimulus. Over subsequent learning experiences the same and similar conditioned responses form into habits, or systems of integrated conditioned responses, which are stereotyped responses that are called up under similar recurring situations. To be serviceable for survival, in Allport’s judgment, the personality unit had to be stable and flexible. To survive it was necessary to learn to behave in response to varied stimuli that are perceived as similar and to adapt to novel conditions. Such demands could not be met by habits. Habits were an inflexible type of response that were evoked by a narrow range of stimulus conditions, in particular situations.

In the hustle and bustle of the human realm of active engagement, particularly in highly complex sociocultural environments, incapacity to respond readily to novelty would not be functionally useful. Malleability was called for. Allport’s preferred unit, therefore, was based on the innate endowment being transformed into habits by conditioning, and, subsequently, the fusion of habits, having the same adaptive significance, into a higher-order system of organization—traits. Once formed, traits were functionally independent of their composite

habits. They ceased being mechanical responses to immediate conditions and, instead, took up an autonomous, directive function in engaging the environment. The stimulus, in this and other developments, as Allport determined, was dethroned. As the developing child passes through new social situations (e.g., home and school) new adaptations and new traits could form. What adaptations are made and the personality that will form depend upon the nature of the social network that the person has to adapt to. This bears upon the issue of national character and is taken up in Allport’s notion of the common trait.

As traits are adaptations to the conditions of one’s existence it stands to reason that having to adapt to the same conditions can result in similar adjustments and, ultimately, similar traits. To the degree that conditions are shared among people, and to the degree that they endure pressure in the direction of culturally approved norms, one can expect similarities in the development of personal traits. Culture, furthermore, generally prescribes acceptable methods and goals in child-rearing. There are pressures within a culture that promote the formation of traits that are basic and common which, by adulthood, may approximate a national character. As Allport conceived of them, common traits are traits that are shared to different degrees by many people and reflect “ those aspects of personality in respect to which most people within a given culture can be profitably compared ” (Allport, 1961, p. 340, emphasis in the original). Cultural similarities, it should be emphasized, do not necessitate or imply group stereotypes. They are comparable adjustments. No two people have exactly the same traits.

Variability exists in the cultural conditions that one can be exposed to and in the style of parenting. Heterogeneity rather than homogeneity is the mark of modern cultures due to a multiplicity of values, beliefs, attitudes, practices, and so on, which will promote the non- uniformity of personalities. There are, on the other hand, cultures that are quite homogeneous and it is to one of these—the Hutterite brethren—that we turn to next in order to concretize Allport’s trait theory and demonstrate a possible basis for notions of national character.

Hutterite Personality Formation

The Hutterites formed in the 16th century and their values and child-rearing practices are based on the traditions which stem from that period (Hostetler, 1970). The world, the Hutterites are taught, is dualistic in nature and its carnal side should be subdued while the spiritual side should be developed. That is the goal of Hutterite socialization. In order for this to be achieved, the individual will had to be broken and individuality suppressed, while self- denial, surrender and subservience to the colony, and obedience to authority should be developed (Hostetler and Huntington, 1967, 1968).

The similarities, over time and across colonies, in the goals and methods of socialization result in a personality that is rather uniform (a national character ):

A successfully socialized Hutterite gets along well with others, is submissive, and obedient to the rules and regulations of the colony, and is a hard-working responsible individual. An adult Hutterite must never display anger nor precipitate quarrels. Intensity and imagination are not admired; rather, quiet willingness coupled with hard work are considered desirable qualities. The constant pruning which adapts each individual to the group results in minimizing of differences and a muting of emotional expression. The elimination of extremes and the imposition of a strict order enable members to find satisfaction in the “narrow way” that leads to salvation. (Hostetler and Huntington, 1968, p. 351)

would become directive in the future. So, for instance, when they begin to attend public school, in a schoolhouse maintained in and by the colony, they can be expected to be immediately deferent, in this new situation, to their new teacher. Admittedly this is not based on any concrete observations but I am disinclined to believe that my speculations are greatly amiss.

Hutterite child-rearing practices have been around for close to five hundred years and have proven effective as a means of developing the ideal, modal Hutterite personality. By that I do not mean exact replicas but a common tendency, a collection of common traits. Hutterite society does not partake of modern social change, given their preference for their ancestral, agrarian collectivism. They prefer a life of greater simplicity over the individualistic, social Darwinian competitiveness, of capitalist economies, and the accompanying complexity of industrial society. The brethren have thus remained a relatively insulated community of believers who largely share in common values. Not all Hutterites accept the colony ways, however. Some defect to the surrounding cultures and adopt non-Hutterite behaviors, adapting to the new conditions, and establishing non-Hutterite personality characteristics. For those who stay—the majority—one can expect the development of the modal personality, as just described, in conformity with the community ideals.

That such modal personalities or stable national characters are possible should not lead us to some false conclusions. Hutterite society has remained stable for centuries and has operated within the framework of a relatively simple social structure. Such a stable social structure, which was an adaptation to a lifestyle from five hundred ago, likely supports the development of a stable and fairly uniform national character. Complex societies, however, as Inkeles (1997) has argued, that are continually evolving and increasing in their complexity, are less likely to support a universal national character. Social complexity, regionalism, ethnicity, social class, education, etc. are likely to result in a greater diffusion of traits and less conformity. Complex industrial societies may be multi-modal because of the multitude of opportunities and the diversity of occupations that individual members can adapt to. Multifariousness of opportunity is unlikely to support homogeneity of national character.

References

Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Henry Holt & Company.

Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Hostetler, J. A. and Huntington, G. E. (1967). The Hutterites in North America. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Hostetler, J. A. and Huntington, G. E. (1968). Communal socialization practices in Hutterite society. Ethnology , 7 , 331‒355.

Inkeles, A. (1997). National character: A psycho-social perspective. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Leont’ev, A. N. (1978). Activity, consciousness, and personality (M. J. Hall, Trans.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher mental functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (144‒188). Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.