










































Study with the several resources on Docsity
Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan
Prepare for your exams
Study with the several resources on Docsity
Earn points to download
Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan
Community
Ask the community for help and clear up your study doubts
Discover the best universities in your country according to Docsity users
Free resources
Download our free guides on studying techniques, anxiety management strategies, and thesis advice from Docsity tutors
TIE-CLIL MODULE 2 - SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FOR CLIL ... 10. Both L1 and L2 learners in the process of learning the English past may ... Some key terms.
Typology: Lecture notes
1 / 50
This page cannot be seen from the preview
Don't miss anything!
MMOODDUULLEE 2 2 SECOND LANGUAGE
ACQUISITION
FOR CLIL
54 TIE-CLIL PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT COURSE
TABLE OF CONTENTS
59 UNIT 1 Interlanguage: Learner’s Language Teresa Navés
67 UNIT 2 Incidental vs. Intentional Learning Maria Pavesi
72 UNIT 3 Input Characteristics Carmen Muñoz
78 UNIT 4 Second Language Competence Maria Pavesi
86 UNIT 5 Individual Characteristics of the Learner Carmen Muñoz
91 UNIT 6 Characteristics of Successful CLIL Programmes Teresa Navés
98 BIBLIOGRAPHY
56 TIE-CLIL PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT COURSE
TIE-CLIL MODULE 2 - SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FOR CLIL 57
Notes to the trainer
1. Before starting the module
TIE-CLIL MODULE 2 - SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FOR CLIL 59
Interlanguage: Learner’s Language 4 hours
60 TIE-CLIL PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT COURSE
UNIT
You may choose one of the following initial activities.
Task 1
In this section you will find out the extent to which some of your expectations and beliefs about (a) how languages are learnt, (b) the type of errors learners make and (c) how teachers should deal with them are either confirmed or disconfirmed by second language acquisition research findings.
Are the following statements true or false? Do you know why?
Initial Evaluation: Checking our own beliefs
62 TIE-CLIL PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT COURSE
UNIT
suggested learners had acquired a given morpheme when they were able to use it grammatically most of the time. Lately, however, most researchers suggest learners have acquired a language form when they are able to use it in spontaneous settings whether in a target-like fashion or not, most of the time. One researcher in particular developed one of the latest models of acquisition based on the idea that a language form had been acquired if learners would use it at least in three obligatory contexts from the communicative tasks in which they were engaged, yet not necessarily target-like. When learners are in the process of learning something they would attempt to use the language form in a wide variety of ways, overgeneralising its rule and also using it in non-obligatory contexts. For example when a learner is in the process of learning the English past , he or she may say I saw you yesterday when I go beach but I didn’t saw Mia. I’m going to called her.
Similarities between First Language and Second Language Acquisition Children do not learn their mother tongue simply through imitation and practice ; instead, they produce utterances that are not like those they have heard. Children’s language seems to be created on the basis of some internal processes and knowledge which interact with the language they are exposed to, allowing them to find out how the language system works gradually. Children’s early language seems best described as a developing system with its own internal and systematic structures, not just an imperfect imitation of the language they are in the process of learning. Finally, children’s language reveals there is an order of acquisition of English morphemes and also some other syntactic structures such as negation. For example, English children invariably first start using the –ing morpheme before they would ever come up with a plural –s form; or they start using the irregular past of some highly frequent verbs such as saw and went before they start using the regular –ed morpheme. When they start using the regular –ed morpheme , they also tend to overgeneralise its use and apart from saying called , they would also say comed. In acquiring English negation , children also go though a series of stages, some of which are not target-like (grammatical). At one stage, English children use pre-verbal negation in utterances like mummy no comb hair. SLA research has also found out that second language learners learn English morphemes in a given order of acquisition and that the plural –s morpheme is acquired much earlier than the third person –s morpheme. In fact, the 3 rd^ person –s morpheme along with the –ed morpheme is one of the latest morphemes acquired by second language learners. Learners will only start using those morphemes in spontaneous situations once they have already acquired other morphemes such as the plural –s.
Interlanguage Interlanguage is the learners’ language, i.e., the type of language produced by learners who are in the process of learning a second language. Interlanguage is also defined as the learners’ developing second language knowledge. Analysis of the learners’ interlanguage shows that it has some characteristics of the learner’s native language, some characteristics of the second language and some characteristics which are very systematic, i.e., rule-governed and common to all learners. In language acquisition, learners’ errors are caused by several different processes.
Learners’ Errors SLA has identified three main types of errors. The first of these are developmental errors, which are similar to the errors made by children learning the language as their first language. Developmental errors are assumed to be a natural product of a gradually developing ability in the new language in the studies so far carried out, developmental errors make up the majority of errors exhibited by second language learners_._ Examples of developmental errors are the misuse of third person –s ( she work hard, he doesn’t works hard), the –ed morpheme ( she teached us last year), of negation ( I not like it) and of interrogatives (I wonder what is she doing). Transfer or interlingual errors, or errors clearly attributable to first language influence, are also frequent. One example of a transfer error for learners from most romance languages learning English is adverb placement in instances like I have every day a cup of coffee in the morning.
TIE-CLIL MODULE 2 - SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FOR CLIL 63
Developmental Sequences Research on SLA has revealed that there are important similarities between first language acquisition and second language acquisition. One important finding has been that in both first and second language acquisition there are sequences or ‘stages’ in the development of particular structures. Developmental sequences are similar across learners from different language backgrounds, from different ages, or from different learning contexts (formal instruction vs. naturalistic). In other words, second and foreign language learners in the process of learning the target language, pass through a series of identifiable stages in acquiring grammatical structures. One such example is provided by Italian tenses where learners irrespectively of mother tongue start by marking completion with past participles. The language that learners produce ( IL ) provides evidence that they acquire different morphological features in a fixed order and also that they pass through a sequence of developmental stages in the acquisition of specific morpho-syntactic features. The existence of developmental sequences is one of the most important findings of SLA research to date. There is a general acceptance in the SLA research community that the acquisition of some features of L2 grammar occurs in stages.
Glossary of related SLA terms from the Tutorial Look for definitions and explanations of the bold-faced terms and italicised terms in the tutorial. Then write definitions for them using your own words. If possible, write down synonyms of those terms.
1 Richards, J. C., Platt, J., and Platt, H., 1992. This well-known dictionary of Applied Linguistics has recently been translated into Spanish: Diccionario de lingüística aplicada y enseñanza de lenguas. Versión española y adaptación de Carmen Muñoz Lahoz y Carmen Pérez Vidal, Editorial Ariel, Barcelona, 1997.
TIE-CLIL MODULE 2 - SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FOR CLIL 65
I no work… I not work … I not want it… She don’t work… She not work
and she attributes these errors to the fact that Spanish as well as Catalan are pre-verbal languages, that is, that negation in Spanish and Catalan is made by placing the negative before the verb.
(Yo) no trabajo….. (Yo) no lo quiero….. (Ella) no trabaja (Jo) no treballo…. (Jo) no ho vull…….. (Ella) no treballa
and concludes that these errors are interlingual errors, that is transfer errors to be attributed to the transfer from learner’s L1 background.
what type of data would you recommend this teacher collect?
of error in learning the verb negation in English?
post-verbal negation (stage 2). The rate is faster although the path is the same. How can we account for it? Is German also a pre-verbal language? If you do not know the answer, what would you predict based on the fact that German learners move sooner from stage 1 to stage 2 in learning verb negation in English?
being engaged in communicative tasks, they were completing a more controlled written grammar exercise for which they had more time to plan and to rely on formal knowledge? Why?
Coffee stains Orange juice stains Oil stains Biological detergent X OK X Non biological detergent OK X OK
S: Non-biological detergent removes coffee stains better than biological detergent. Non-biological detergent remove stains in oil better. Non-biological detergent don’t remove orange juice well
66 TIE-CLIL PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT COURSE
UNIT
Lightbown, P. & Spada, N., “Learner Language”, in How languages are learned , Oxford University Press,1994: 71-90.
Larsen-Freeman, Diane “Grammar and Its Teaching: Challenging the Myths”. ERIC Digest. Source: ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics Washington DC. http://www.ed.gov/databases/ERIC_Digests/ed406829.html
Choose the best answers and justify them in the right-hand column
68 TIE-CLIL PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT COURSE
UNIT
68
Group work
focused or content-focused learning, e.g., when using the language for reading course textbooks or professional material, learning grammar rules, practising apologising in role playing activities, listening to radio broadcasts in a foreign language, etc.
Two types of L2 knowledge are commonly discussed in second language acquisition: explicit knowledge and implicit knowledge. Explicit knowledge is made up of features of which the learner is aware and which s/he is able to verbalise. Implicit knowledge is intuitive and mostly works at an unconscious level. Explicit knowledge is often the initial result of language teaching, when the language input is manipulated by the teacher who presents some specific grammar rules, vocabulary items or any other language facts. In such cases learners pay conscious attention to what they are learning of the target language and they become conscious of what they know. Provided the focus of attention is on language, explicit knowledge develops independently of manner of presentation. Implicit knowledge can become explicit knowledge when, for example, learners analyse and reflect on some linguistic facts they implicitly know. This is what typically also occurs when native speakers recognise patterns and regularities in their automatic linguistic behaviour. For example, they may at times come up with an intuitive rule which accounts for their use of the definite article in English or the passé composé in French. Explicit language learning is thus necessarily intentional. It relies on the same general cognitive processes that underlie the learning of other types of knowledge such as history or mathematics. Two main processes seem to be responsible for the internalising of such knowledge: problem-solving
TIE-CLIL MODULE 2 - SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FOR CLIL 69
Of course, awareness about language may naturally arise at any moment during the process. It is in fact quite difficult and fundamentally unproductive to separate between explicit and implicit learning. As with most human behaviours it is a matter of degree and as such it must be conceived during actual teaching. Language learning that derives from content teaching through a foreign language strongly relies on incidental learning, which is why immersion and CLIL programmes are welcomed as excellent opportunities for language learning. In this regard, it has been observed that “some of the most effective learning comes peripherally, ‘out of the corner of one’s eye’”, but the learner must be made aware that “learning is really taking place, even though it is not directly controlled” (Ehrman, 1996:183). Intentional learning, on the other hand, obviously remains the main objective of formal language teaching. In Krashen’s Input Hypothesis explicit learning plays only a limited role as a monitor, that is, a language corrector that operates when time is available, the rule is known and attention is on language rather than meaning. Recent research on second language acquisition, however, suggests that, given the right conditions, explicit learning can also help in developing implicit language knowledge. Moreover, explicit knowledge of some language items can incidentally help notice and thus internalise these items from the input. In this way explicit knowledge acts indirectly on implicit learning.
1) Discussion on which of the following activities foster incidental or intentional learning of the target language
2) Task for language teachers
In language teaching methodology a distinction is often drawn between so called deductive and inductive approaches. In deductive language teaching, rules and generalisations about linguistic behaviour are presented first to be applied and practised later. In inductive teaching, learners are presented with selected instances of language from which they are asked to extract regularities.
Do the two approaches differ in terms of focusing on content or language? Do they promote implicit or explicit knowledge?
TIE-CLIL MODULE 2 - SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FOR CLIL 71
While discussing Ellis’s position, participants should:
English negation and interrogation). Provide examples of areas that could benefit from explicit learning;
structures such as the cell membrane surrounds the cell, the nuclear membrane surrounds the nucleus ).
6) Viewing task for content teachers (individual or group work)
Videos or extracts from videos where some instances of focus-on-language conducing to intentional or explicit learning are shown. Participants should identify which aspects of language are more likely to be treated explicitly in a CLIL class.
MAIN POINT In CLIL contexts content teachers often draw students’ attention to the meaning and use of technical vocabulary, sometimes to pronunciation, very rarely to grammar rules.
Reference text : Ellis, R., SLA Research and Language Teaching , Chap. 4: A theory of instructed second language acquisition, Oxford University Press, 1997: 107-133. (Individual work and class discussion)
ideas, concepts, notions that are more relevant to CLIL (listing, note-taking, drawing tree-diagrams, etc.)
discussed in Ellis and relevant to CLIL.
Formative assessment task (individual work): Answer the following questions in writing:
72 TIE-CLIL PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT COURSE
UNIT
72
Input Characteristics 4 hours