Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Stress and Coping Theories, Study Guides, Projects, Research of Stress Analysis

This article first presents two theories representing distinct approaches to the field of stress research.

Typology: Study Guides, Projects, Research

2021/2022

Uploaded on 01/21/2022

millyx
millyx 🇺🇸

4.7

(9)

249 documents

1 / 13

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
Stress and Coping Theories
H. W. Krohnea
a Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz Germany
Available online 2002.
Abstract
This article first presents two theories representing distinct approaches to the field of stress
research: Selye's theory of `systemic stress' based in physiology and psychobiology, and the
`psychological stress' model developed by Lazarus. In the second part, the concept of coping
is described. Coping theories may be classified according to two independent parameters:
trait- oriented versus state-oriented, and microanalytic versus macroanalytic approaches. The
multitude of theoretical conceptions is based on the macroanalytic, trait-oriented approach.
Examples of this approach that are presented in this article are `repression–sensitization,'
`monitoring- blunting,' and the `model of coping modes.' The article closes with a brief
outline of future perspectives in stress and coping research.
Article Outline
1. Theories of Stress
1.1. Systemic Stress: Selye's Theory
1.2. Psychological Stress: The Lazarus Theory
1.3. Resource Theories of Stress: A Bridge between Systemic and Cognitive Viewpoints
2. Coping Theories
2.1. Classification of Approaches
2.2. Macroanalytic, Trait-Oriented Coping Theories
2.2.1. Repression–sensitization.
2.2.2. Monitoring and blunting.
2.2.3. The model of coping modes.
3. Future Perspectives
References
For the last five decades the term stress has enjoyed increasing popularity in the behavioral
and health sciences. It first was used in physics in order to analyze the problem of how man-
made structures must be designed to carry heavy loadsand resist deformation by external
focus. In this analysis, stress referred to external pressure or force applied to a structure, while
strain denoted the resulting internal distortion of the object (for the term's history, cf. Hinkle
1974, Mason 1975a, 1975c). In the transition from physics to the behavioral sciences, the
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa
pfd

Partial preview of the text

Download Stress and Coping Theories and more Study Guides, Projects, Research Stress Analysis in PDF only on Docsity!

Stress and Coping Theories

H. W. Krohne a

a (^) Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz Germany

Available online 2002.

Abstract

This article first presents two theories representing distinct approaches to the field of stress research: Selye's theory of systemic stress' based in physiology and psychobiology, and thepsychological stress' model developed by Lazarus. In the second part, the concept of coping is described. Coping theories may be classified according to two independent parameters: trait- oriented versus state-oriented, and microanalytic versus macroanalytic approaches. The multitude of theoretical conceptions is based on the macroanalytic, trait-oriented approach. Examples of this approach that are presented in this article are repression–sensitization,'monitoring- blunting,' and the `model of coping modes.' The article closes with a brief outline of future perspectives in stress and coping research.

Article Outline

  1. Theories of Stress 1.1. Systemic Stress: Selye's Theory 1.2. Psychological Stress: The Lazarus Theory 1.3. Resource Theories of Stress: A Bridge between Systemic and Cognitive Viewpoints
  2. Coping Theories 2.1. Classification of Approaches 2.2. Macroanalytic, Trait-Oriented Coping Theories 2.2.1. Repression–sensitization. 2.2.2. Monitoring and blunting. 2.2.3. The model of coping modes.
  3. Future Perspectives References

For the last five decades the term stress has enjoyed increasing popularity in the behavioral and health sciences. It first was used in physics in order to analyze the problem of how man- made structures must be designed to carry heavy loadsand resist deformation by external focus. In this analysis, stress referred to external pressure or force applied to a structure, while strain denoted the resulting internal distortion of the object (for the term's history, cf. Hinkle 1974, Mason 1975a, 1975c). In the transition from physics to the behavioral sciences, the

usage of the term stress changed. In most approaches it now designates bodily processes created by circumstances that place physical or psychological demands on an individual (Selye 1976). The external forces that impinge on the body are called stressors (McGrath 1982).

1. Theories of Stress

Theories that focus on the specific relationship between external demands (stressors) and bodily processes (stress) can be grouped in two different categories: approaches to systemic stress' based in physiology and psychobiology (among others, Selye 1976) and approaches topsychological stress' developed within the field of cognitive psychology (Lazarus 1966, 1991, Lazarus and Folkman 1984, McGrath 1982).

1.1. Systemic Stress: Selye's Theory

The popularity of the stress concept in science and mass media stems largely from the work of the endocrinologist Hans Selye. In a series of animal studies he observed that a variety of stimulus events (e.g., heat, cold, toxic agents) applied intensely and long enough are capable of producing common effects, meaning not specific to either stimulus event. (Besides these nonspecific changes in the body, each stimulus produces, of course, its specific effect, heat, for example, produces vasodilatation, and cold vasoconstriction.) According to Selye, these nonspecifically caused changes constitute the stereotypical, i.e., specific , response pattern of systemic stress. Selye ( 1976, p. 64) defines this stress as `a state manifested by a syndrome which consists of all the nonspecifically induced changes in a biologic system.'

This stereotypical response pattern, called the `General Adaptation Syndrome' (GAS), proceeds in three stages. (a) The alarm reaction comprises an initial shock phase and a subsequent countershock phase. The shock phase exhibits autonomic excitability, an increased adrenaline discharge, and gastro-intestinal ulcerations. The countershock phase marks the initial operation of defensive processes and is characterized by increased adrenocortical activity. (b) If noxious stimulation continues, the organism enters the stage of resistance. In this stage, the symptoms of the alarm reaction disappear, which seemingly indicates the organism's adaptation to the stressor. However, while resistance to the noxious stimulation increases, resistance to other kinds of stressors decreases at the same time. (c) Ifthe aversive stimulation persists, resistance gives way to the stage of exhaustion. The organism's capability of adapting to the stressor is exhausted, the symptoms of stage (a) reappear, but resistance is no longer possible. Irreversible tissue damages appear, and, if the stimulation persists, the organism dies.

Although Selye's work influenced a whole generation of stress researchers, marked weaknesses in his theory soon became obvious. First of all, Selye's conception of stress as a reaction to a multitude of different events had the fatal consequence that the stress concept became the melting pot for all kinds of approaches. Thus, by becoming a synonym for diverse terms such as, for example, anxiety, threat, conflict, or emotional arousal, the concept of stress was in danger of losing its scientific value (cf. Engel 1985). Besides this general reservation, specific critical issues have been raised. One criticism was directed at the theory's core assumption of a nonspecific causation of the GAS. Mason ( 1971, 1975b) pointed out that the stressors observed as effective by Selye carried a common emotional meaning: they were novel, strange, and unfamiliar to the animal. Thus, the animal's state could be described in terms of helplessness, uncertainty, and lack of control. Consequently, the hormonal GAS responses followed the (specific) emotional impact of such influences rather than the

forms rely on different sources of information. Primary appraisal concerns whether something of relevance to the individual's well being occurs,whereas secondary appraisal concerns coping options.

Within primary appraisal , three components are distinguished: goal relevance describes the extent to which an encounter refers to issues about which the person cares. Goal congruence defines the extent to whichan episode proceeds in accordance with personal goals. Type of ego- involvement designates aspects of personal commitment such as self- esteem, moral values, ego-ideal, or ego-identity. Likewise, three secondary appraisal components are distinguished: blame or credit results from an individual's appraisal of who is responsible for a certain event. By coping potential Lazarus means a person's evaluation of the prospects for generating certain behavioral or cognitive operations that will positively influence a personally relevant encounter. Future expectations refer to the appraisal of the further course of an encounter with respect to goal congruence or incongruence.

Specific patterns of primary and secondary appraisal lead to different kinds of stress. Three types are distinguished: harm, threat, and challenge (Lazarus and Folkman 1984). Harm refers to the (psychological) damage or loss that has already happened. Threat is the anticipation of harm that may be imminent. Challenge results from demands that a person feels confident about mastering. These different kinds of psychological stress are embedded in specific types of emotional reactions, thus illustrating the close conjunction of the fields of stress and emotions.

Lazarus ( 1991) distinguishes 15 basic emotions. Nine of these are negative (anger, fright, anxiety, guilt, shame, sadness, envy, jealousy, and disgust), whereas four are positive (happiness, pride, relief, and love). (Two more emotions, hope and compassion, have a mixed valence.) At a molecular level of analysis, the anxiety reaction, for example, is based on the following pattern of primary and secondary appraisals: there must be some goal relevance to the encounter. Furthermore, goal incongruence is high, i.e., personal goals are thwarted. Finally, ego- involvement concentrates on the protection of personal meaning or ego- identity against existential threats. At a more molar level, specific appraisal patterns related to stress or distinct emotional reactions are described as core relational themes. The theme of anxiety, for example, is the confrontation with uncertainty and existential threat. The core relational theme of relief, however, is `a distressing goal-incongruent condition that has changed for the better or gone away' (Lazarus 1991).

Coping is intimately related to the concept of cognitive appraisal and, hence, to the stress- relevant person-environment transactions. Most approaches in coping research follow Folkman and Lazarus (1980, p. 223), who define coping as `the cognitive and behavioral efforts made to master, tolerate, or reduce external and internal demands and conflicts among them.'

This definition contains the following implications. (a) Coping actions are not classified according to their effects (e.g., as reality-distorting), but according to certain characteristics of the coping process. (b) This process encompasses behavioral as well as cognitive reactions in the individual. (c) In most cases, coping consists of different single acts andis organized sequentially, forming a coping episode. In this sense, coping is often characterized by the simultaneous occurrence of different action sequences and, hence, an interconnection of coping episodes. (d) Coping actions can be distinguished by their focus on different elements of a stressful encounter (cf. Lazarus and Folkman 1984 ). They can attempt to change the person–environment realities behind negative emotions or stress ( problem-focused coping ).

They can also relate to internal elements and try to reduce a negative emotional state, or change the appraisal of the demanding situation ( emotion-focused coping ).

1.3. Resource Theories of Stress: A Bridge between Systemic and Cognitive Viewpoints

Unlike approaches discussed so far, resource theories of stress are not primarily concerned with factors that create stress, but with resources that preserve well being in the face of stressful encounters. Several social and personal constructs have been proposed, such as social support (Schwarzer and Leppin 1991), sense of coherence (Antonovsky 1979), hardiness (Kobasa 1979), self-efficacy (Bandura 1977), or optimism (Scheier and Carver 1992). Whereas self-efficacy and optimism are single protective factors, hardiness and sense of coherence represent tripartite approaches. Hardiness is an amalgam of three components: internal control, commitment, and a sense of challenge as opposed to threat. Similarly, sense of coherence consists of believing that the world is meaningful, predictable, and basically benevolent. Within the social support field, several types have been investigated, such as instrumental, informational, appraisal, and emotional support.

The recently offered conservation of resources (COR) theory (Hobfoll 1989, Hobfoll et al.

  1. assumes that stress occurs in any of three contexts: when people experience loss of resources, when resources are threatened, or when people invest their resources without subsequent gain. Four categories of resources are proposed: object resources (i.e., physical objects such as home, clothing, or access to transportation), condition resources (e.g., employment, personal relationships), personal resources (e.g., skills or self-efficacy), and energy resources (means that facilitate the attainment of other resources, for example, money, credit, or knowledge).

Hobfoll and co-workers outlined a number of testable hypotheses (called principles) derived from basic assumptions of COR (cf. Hobfoll et al. 1996).

  1. Loss of resources is the primary source of stress. This principle contradicts the fundamental assumption of approaches on critical life events (cf. Holmes and Rahe 1967) that stress occurs whenever individuals are forced to readjust themselves to situational circumstances, may these circumstances be positive (e.g., marriage) or negative (e.g., loss of a beloved person). In an empirical test of this basic principle, Hobfoll and Lilly ( 1993) found that only loss of resources wasrelated to distress.
  2. Resources act to preserve and protect other resources. Self-esteem is an important resource that may be beneficial for other resources. Hobfoll and Leiberman ( 1987), for example, observed that women who were high in self-esteem made good use of social support when confronted with stress, whereas those who lacked self-esteem interpreted social support as an indication of personal inadequacy and, consequently, misused support.
  3. Following stressful circumstances, individuals have an increasingly depleted resource pool to combat further stress. This depletion impairs individuals' capability of coping with further stress, thus resulting in a loss spiral. This process view of resource investment requires to focus on how the interplay between resources and situational demands changes over time as stressor sequences unfold. In addition, this principle shows that it is important to investigate not only the effect of resources on outcome, but also of outcome on resources.

2. Coping Theories

regard to the relationship between these two constructs, Byrne's approach specifies a unidimensional, bipolar structure, while Miller as well as Mullen and Suls leave this question open. Krohne, however, explicitly postulates an independent functioning of the dimensions vigilance and cognitive avoidance.

2.2.1. Repression–sensitization.

The repression–sensitization construct (cf. Byrne 1964, Eriksen 1966) relates different forms of dispositional coping to one bipolar dimension. When confronted with a stressful encounter, persons located at one pole of this dimension (repressers) tend to deny or minimize the existence of stress, fail to verbalize feelings of distress, and avoid thinking about possible negative consequences of this encounter. Persons at the opposite pole (sensitizers) react to stress-related cues by way of enhanced information search, rumination, and obsessive worrying. The concept of repression–sensitization is theoretically founded in research on perceptual defense (Bruner and Postman 1947), an approach that combined psychodynamic ideas with the functionalistic behavior analysis of Brunswik ( 1947).

2.2.2. Monitoring and blunting.

The conception of monitoring and blunting (Miller 1980, 1987) originated from the same basic assumptions formulated earlier by Eriksen ( 1966) for the repression–sensitization construct. Miller conceived both constructs as cognitive informational styles and proposed that individuals who encounter a stressful situation react with arousal according to the amount of attention they direct to the stressor. Conversely, the arousal level can be lowered, if the person succeeds in reducing the impact of aversive cues by employing avoidant cognitive strategies such as distraction, denial, or reinterpretation. However, these coping strategies, called blunting, should only be adaptive if the aversive event is uncontrollable. Examples of uncontrollable events are impending surgery or an aversive medical examination (Miller and Mangan 1983). If control is available, strategies called monitoring, i.e., seeking information about the stressor, are the more adaptive forms of coping. Although initially these strategies are associated with increased stress reactions, they enable the individual to gain control over the stressor in the long run, thus reducing the impact of the stressful situation. An example of a more controllable stressor is preparing for an academic exam.

The general relationship between a stressor's degree of controllability and the employment of monitoring or blunting strategies can be moderated by situative and personal influences. With regard to situation, the noxious stimulation may be so intense that blunting strategies, such as attentional diversion, are ineffective with respect to reducing stress-related arousal. Concerning personality, there are relatively stable individual differences in the inclination to employ blunting or monitoring coping when encountering a stressor.

2.2.3. The model of coping modes.

Similar to Miller's monitoring-blunting conception, the model of coping modes (MCM) deals with individual differences in attention orientation and emotional- behavioral regulation under stressful conditions (Krohne 1993). The MCM extends the (largely descriptive) monitoring- blunting conception (as well as the repression–sensitization approach) in that it relates the dimensions vigilance and cognitive avoidance to an explicative cognitive-motivational basis. It assumes that most stressful, especially anxiety evoking, situations are characterized by two central features: the presence of aversive stimulation and a high degree of ambiguity. The experiential counterparts of these situational features are emotional arousal (as being primarily related to aversive stimulation) and uncertainty (related to ambiguity). Arousal, in

turn, should stimulate the tendency to cognitively avoid (or inhibit) the further processing of cues related to the aversive encounter, whereas uncertainty activates vigilant tendencies.

These two coping processes are conceptually linked to personality by the hypothesis that the habitual preference for avoidant or vigilant coping strategies reflects individual differences in the susceptibility to emotional arousal or uncertainty. Individuals who are especially susceptible to states of stress-induced emotional arousal are supposed to habitually employ cognitive avoidance. The employment of avoidant strategies primarily aims at shielding the person from an increase in arousal ( arousal-motivated coping behavior ). Individuals who are especially affected by the uncertainty experienced in most stressful situations are supposed to habitually employ vigilant coping. Thus, the employment of vigilant strategies follows a plan that is aimed at minimizing the probability of unanticipated occurrence of aversive events ( uncertainty-motivated coping behavior ).

The MCM conceives the habitual coping tendencies of vigilance and cognitive avoidance as independent personality dimensions. That means, aggregated across a multitude of stressful encounters, the employment of vigilant strategies and of avoidant ones does not preclude each other. Thus, four coping modes can be defined. (a) Persons who score high on vigilance and low on cognitive avoidance are called sensitizers. These persons are primarily concerned with reducing uncertainty by directing their attention towards stress- relevant information. (b) Individuals with the opposite pattern are designated as repressers. These persons minimize the experience of arousal by avoiding aversive information. (c) Nondefensives have low scores on both dimensions. These persons are supposed to flexibly adapt to the demands of a stressful encounter. Instead of frequently employing vigilant or avoidant coping strategies, they prefer to act instrumentally in most situations. (d) Individuals who exhibit high scores on both dimensions are called high anxious. In employing vigilant as well as avoidant coping strategies, these persons try to reduce both the subjective uncertainty and the emotional arousal induced by stressful encounters. Because the two goals are incompatible in most situations, high-anxious persons are assumed to show fluctuating and therefore less-efficient coping behavior. Approaches to assess individual differences in vigilance and cognitive avoidance are described in Krohne et al. ( 2000). Empirical results related to predictions derived from the MCM are presented in Krohne ( 1993, 1996), and Krohne et al. ( 1992).

3. Future Perspectives

Although the fields of stress and coping research represent largely explored territory, there are still fertile perspectives to be pursued in future research. Among the promising lines of research, two perspectives will be mentioned here.

  1. Compared to the simplistic stimulus-response conception of stress inherent in early approaches on stress, the psychological' (i.e., cognitive transformation) approach of the Lazarus group clearly represents progress. However, in advocating a completelysubjective' orientation in conceptualizing stress, Lazarus overstated the cognitive turn' in stress research. In stating thatwe might do better by describing relevant environments and their psychological meanings through the lenses of individuals' (Lazarus 1990, p. 8) he took a stand that is at variance with the multivariate, systems- theory perspective proposed in his recent publications on stress and emotions (Lazarus 1990, 1991).

First, the stress process contains variables to be assessed both subjectively and objectively, such as constraints, temporal aspects, or social support networks, as well as responses to be measured at different levels (cf. Lazarus 1990, Table 1). Second, the fact that most objective

Arnold, M B, (1960). Emotion and Personality (2 Vols.). New York: Columbia University Press.

Bandura, A, (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review , 84 , pp. 191–215. Abstract-MEDLINE | Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Bruner, J S and Postman, L, (1947). Emotional selectivity in perception and reaction. Journal of Personality , 16 , pp. 69–77. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Brunswik, E, (1947). Systematic and Representative Design of Psychological Experiments: With Results in Physical and Social Perception. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Byrne, D, (1964). Repression–sensitization as a dimension of personality. In B A Maher (Ed), (1964). Progress in Experimental Personality Research (pp. 169–220). New York, Vol. 1,: Academic Press.

Engel, B T, (1985). Stress is a noun! No, a verb! No, an adjective. In T M Field, P M McCabe and N Schneiderman (Eds), (1985). Stress and Coping (pp. 3–12). Hillsdale, NJ,: Erlbaum.

Eriksen, C W, (1966). Cognitive responses to internally cued anxiety. In C D Spielberger (Ed), (1966). Anxiety and Behavior (pp. 327–360). New York,: Academic Press.

Folkman, S and Lazarus, R S, (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior , 21 , pp. 219–239. Abstract-MEDLINE | Abstract-EMBASE | Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Folkman, S and Lazarus, R S, (1988). Ways of Coping Questionnaire Research edition. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Freud, A, (1936). Das Ich und die Abwehrmechanismen [The ego and the mechanisms of defense]. Vienna: Internationaler Psychoanalytischer Verlag.

Freud, S, (1926). Hemmung, Symptom und Angst [Inhibitions, symptoms and anxiety]. Vienna: Internationaler Psychoanalytischer Verlag.

Hinkle, L E, (1974). The concept of `stress' in the biological and social sciences. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine , 5 , pp. 335–357. Abstract-PsycINFO | Abstract-MEDLINE | $Order Document

Hobfoll, S E, (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist , 44 , pp. 513–524. Abstract | Abstract + References | PDF (1354 K)

Hobfoll, S E, Freedy, J R, Green, B L and Solomon, S D, (1996). Coping reactions to extreme stress: The roles of resource loss and resource availability. In M Zeidner and N S Endler (Eds), (1996). Handbook of Coping: Theory, Research, Applications (pp. 322–349). New York,: Wiley. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Hobfoll, S E and Leiberman, J R, (1987). Personality and social resources in immediate and continued stress-resistance among women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 52 , pp. 18–26. Abstract-MEDLINE | Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Hobfoll, S E and Lilly, R S, (1993). Resource conservation as a strategy for community psychology. Journal of Community Psychology , 21 , pp. 128–148. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Holmes, T H and Rahe, R H, (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research , 11 , pp. 213–218. Abstract | Abstract + References | PDF (430 K)

Janis, I L, (1958). Psychological Stress: Psychoanalytic and Behavioral Studies of Surgical Patients. New York: Wiley.

Janis, I L, (1983). Stress inoculation in health care: Theory and research. In D Meichenbaum and M Jaremko (Eds), (1983). Stress Reduction and Prevention (pp. 67–99). New York,: Plenum.

Karoly, P, (1999). A goal systems self-regulatory perspective on personality, psychopathology, and change. Review of General Psychology , 3 , pp. 264–291. Abstract | Abstract + References | PDF (2359 K)

Kobasa, S C, (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 37 , pp. 1–11. Abstract-MEDLINE | Abstract- PsycINFO | $Order Document

Kohlmann, C-W, (1997). Persönlichkeit und Emotionsregulation: Defensive Bewältigung von Angst und Streß [Personality and the regulation of emotion]. Bern, Switzerland: Huber.

Krohne, H W, (1978). Individual differences in coping with stress and anxiety. In C D Spielberger and I G Sarason (Eds), (1978). Stress and Anxiety (pp. 233–260). Washington, DC, Vol. 5,: Hemisphere.

Krohne, H W, (1993). Vigilance and cognitive avoidance as concepts in coping research. In H W Krohne (Ed), (1993). Attention and Avoidance. Strategies in Coping with Aversiveness (pp. 19–50). Seattle, WA,: Hogrefe & Huber. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Krohne, H W, (1996). Individual differences in coping. In M Zeidner and N S Endler (Eds), (1996). Handbook of Coping: Theory, Research, Applications (pp. 381–409). New York,: Wiley. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Krohne, H W, Egloff, B, Varner, L J, Burns, L R, Weidner, G and Ellis, H C, (2000). The assessment of dispositional vigilance and cognitive avoidance: Factorial structure, psychometric properties, and validity of the Mainz Coping Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research , 24 , pp. 297–311. Abstract-EMBASE | Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document | Full Text via CrossRef

Krohne, H W, Hock, M and Kohlmann, C-W, (1992). Coping dispositions, uncertainty, and emotional arousal. In K T Strongman (Ed), (1992). International Review of Studies on Emotion (pp. 73–95). Chichester, UK, Vol. 2,: Wiley. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Lazarus, R S, (1966). Psychological Stress and the Coping Process. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Mischel, W and Shoda, Y, (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review , 102 , pp. 248–268.

Mullen, B and Suls, J, (1982). The effectiveness of attention and rejection as coping styles: A meta-analysis of temporal differences. Journal of Psychosomatic Research , 26 , pp. 43–49. Abstract | Abstract + References | PDF (532 K)

Roth, S and Cohen, L J, (1986). Approach, avoidance, and coping with stress. American Psychologist , 41 , pp. 813–819. Abstract-PsycINFO | Abstract-MEDLINE | $Order Document

Scheier, M F and Carver, C S, (1992). Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and Research , 16 , pp. 201–228. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Schwarzer, R and Leppin, A, (1991). Social support and health: A theoretical and empirical overview. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships , 8 , pp. 99–127. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Schwarzer, R and Schwarzer, C, (1996). A critical survey of coping instruments. In M Zeidner and N S Endler (Eds), (1996). Handbook of Coping: Theory, Research, Applications (pp. 107–132). New York,: Wiley. Abstract-PsycINFO | $Order Document

Selye, H, (1976). The Stress of Life (rev. edn.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Thoits, P A, (1983). Dimensions of life events that influence psychological distress: An evaluation and synthesis of the literature. In H B Kaplan (Ed), (1983). Psychosocial Stress: Trends in Theory and Research (pp. 33–103). New York,: Academic Press.

M Zeidner and N S Endler (Eds), (1996). Handbook of Coping: Theory, Research, Applications New York: Wiley.