Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Understanding Human Happiness: Reviewing Psychological, Economic, and Behavioral Studies, Summaries of Psychology

An overview of the scientific research on human happiness, drawing from various fields including psychology, business leadership, economics, and behavioral economics. The paper explores the concept of subjective well-being and happiness through psychological theories such as tension-reduction, goal attainment, activity and process, and genetic and biologic dispositional theory. The document also discusses the concept of hedonic adaptation and the comprehensive model of happiness proposed by Lyubomirsky and Layous.

What you will learn

  • How can intentional activities boost happiness levels?
  • What is the comprehensive model of happiness proposed by Lyubomirsky and Layous?
  • How does hedonic adaptation impact happiness?
  • What role do genetics and biology play in happiness?
  • What are the psychological theories of happiness?

Typology: Summaries

2020/2021

Uploaded on 05/24/2021

ekaashaah
ekaashaah 🇺🇸

4.4

(40)

274 documents

1 / 15

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal
92
2013 Volume 4
The Psychology and Science of Happiness: What Does the
Research Say?
David Dubner
Abstract
This paper surveys the scientific evidence regarding the nature of human happiness by drawing upon
numerous studies in social science domains: namely psychology, but also related fields of business
leadership and consulting, economics, and behavioral economics. Beginning with a definition and
narrowing of the terminology, the review moves toward a brief sketch of measurement methods in the
topic. Subjective well-being and happiness are then explored within the framework of psychological
theories of happiness, including tension-reduction and goal attainment theories, activity and process
theories, and genetic and biologic dispositional theory. Evolving views regarding hedonic adaptation as a
key process underlying happiness is examined, as well as Lyubomirsky and Layous’ (2013)
comprehensive model of happiness as a framework that encompasses the entire scope of psychological
theories regarding happiness. Finally, the correlational evidence related to the various components of
happiness is reviewed, concluding with the observation that although strong correlational evidence is
linked to well-being, happiness is a concept that defies unequivocal causal attributions because it
contains a complex amalgam of interrelated and bi-reciprocal variables.
Defining Happiness
Any serious discussion about happiness must first begin by attempting to define what
we mean by the term happiness. In general, happiness can be thought of as an
emotional state that reflects a high level of mental and/or emotional well-being. Current
scientific perspectives typically frame happiness as a complex binary construct that
encompasses subjective elements of both affect and cognition that contribute to well-
being. Subjective well-being (SWB), according to Lyubomirsky and Dickerhoof (2006)
represents people’s beliefs and feelings about whether they are leading a rewarding
and desirable life (p. 167).” Diener, Oishi, and Lucas (2009), define SWB as “
a person’s cognitive and affective evaluations of his or her life as a whole (p.187),”
including evaluations of emotional reactions to life events, and also cognitive judgments
David G Dubner is a doctoral student in Counseling Psychology at Northern Arizona University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to David.Dubner@nau.edu.
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa
pfd
pfe
pff

Partial preview of the text

Download Understanding Human Happiness: Reviewing Psychological, Economic, and Behavioral Studies and more Summaries Psychology in PDF only on Docsity!

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 92

The Psychology and Science of Happiness: What Does the

Research Say?

David Dubner

Abstract This paper surveys the scientific evidence regarding the nature of human happiness by drawing upon numerous studies in social science domains: namely psychology, but also related fields of business leadership and consulting, economics, and behavioral economics. Beginning with a definition and narrowing of the terminology, the review moves toward a brief sketch of measurement methods in the topic. Subjective well-being and happiness are then explored within the framework of psychological theories of happiness, including tension-reduction and goal attainment theories, activity and process theories, and genetic and biologic dispositional theory. Evolving views regarding hedonic adaptation as a key process underlying happiness is examined, as well as Lyubomirsky and Layous’ (2013) comprehensive model of happiness as a framework that encompasses the entire scope of psychological theories regarding happiness. Finally, the correlational evidence related to the various components of happiness is reviewed, concluding with the observation that although strong correlational evidence is linked to well-being, happiness is a concept that defies unequivocal causal attributions because it contains a complex amalgam of interrelated and bi-reciprocal variables. Defining Happiness Any serious discussion about happiness must first begin by attempting to define what we mean by the term happiness. In general, happiness can be thought of as an emotional state that reflects a high level of mental and/or emotional well-being. Current scientific perspectives typically frame happiness as a complex binary construct that encompasses subjective elements of both affect and cognition that contribute to well- being. Subjective well-being (SWB), according to Lyubomirsky and Dickerhoof (2006) “ represents people’s beliefs and feelings about whether they are leading a rewarding and desirable life (p. 167).” Diener, Oishi, and Lucas (2009), define SWB as “ a person’s cognitive and affective evaluations of his or her life as a whole (p.187),” including evaluations of emotional reactions to life events, and also cognitive judgments David G Dubner is a doctoral student in Counseling Psychology at Northern Arizona University. Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to David.Dubner@nau.edu.

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 93 about satisfaction and fulfillment, and especially in important life domains, such as marriage and work (Diener, 2012). The roots of this dichotomous view of happiness can be traced to discourse that philosophers and religious scholars have engaged in throughout the ages as they have long pondered the nature, causes, and methods of fostering human happiness. The idea of happiness as personal fulfillment; eudaimonia, and the idea of happiness as pleasure; hedonia are rooted in the classical Greek philosophy of Aristotle’s Nichomachean Ethics (Waterman, 2013). Aristotle eschewed the idea that the seeking of pleasure (hedonism) is a worthwhile pursuit and ascribed true happiness to eudaimonia a path toward what he called the good life, which is characterized by a focus on virtuous activities that reflect the best within us. Eudaimonia is translated from the Greek to mean “flourishing”, and can be defined as the subjective feeling of personal fulfillment that arises when our actions have reflected our highest potentials. The positive psychology movement, which has its own historical roots in the human potential movement of Rogers and Maslow, has unabashedly adopted the Aristotelian concepts of hedonia and eudaimonia in an attempt to frame the exploration of happiness within the field of psychology. Indeed, positive psychology defines itself as the scientific study of human flourishing (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000), and Martin Seligman’s (the oft-noted founder of the positive psychology movement) theoretical model of happiness is based on the good life which contains three elements: the Pleasant Life, the Meaningful Life, and the Engaged Life (Seligman, Rashid & Parks, 2006). The Aristotelian notion that virtuous actions are necessary for true human happiness has been echoed in religious teachings from the Buddhist notion of Nirvana to the Catholic theologian Thomas Aquinas’s description of felicity as a blessed happiness which requires virtuous acts in accordance with natural and divine law. In contemporary models of happiness espoused by positive psychologists, virtues are also known as character strengths, and considered to be a royal road toward increasing personal well-being and happiness. The VIA (Values-In-Action) classification system (Peterson & Park, 2009) and the Clifton StrengthFinder (Clifton Strengths School, n.d.)

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 95 personal strengths as a means to happiness) was developed as a result of the life’s work of Donald O. Clifton. Clifton’s seminal business organization Selection Research, Inc. eventually acquired Gallup, with Clifton leading the team of researchers from 1988- 1999 (Clifton Strengths School, n.d.). Currently, Gallup Inc. is at the forefront of demographical national and global research that aims to delineate the dynamic nature of well-being with its U.S. National Well-Being Index and Gallup World Poll (Gallup, n.d.). Drawing on these data gathered by the Gallup World Poll (and other global data), specific information about systematic differences between the global well-being of nations and their putative causes has been summarized in the comprehensive World Happiness Report by Columbia University’s Earth Institute. According to the report, the happiest countries in the world are in Northern Europe (Denmark, Finland, Norway), while the least happy are in sub-Saharan African nations (Central African Republic, Benin, Togo). The United States was ranked 11th^ happiest. The report’s authors conclude that aside from income levels, crucial factors correlating to higher well-being include low political corruption, strong social networks, and political freedom. On the individual level, the correlates of high well-being are mental and physical health, social support, stable family structure, and a secure job (The World Happiness Report, n.d.). Measuring Happiness Research conducted by the Gallup organization is representative of how happiness is often measured. Individuals are surveyed about their levels of well-being and the information is compiled to develop national indices. The World Happiness Report states that “among various measures of subjective well-being, the primary distinction to be made is between cognitive life evaluations (represented by questions asking how happy or satisfied people are with their lives as a whole), and emotional reports” (The World Happiness Report, p. 11, n.d.). Emotional reports are further distinguished from those that assess current emotional status (how happy are you now) and those that are retrospective (how happy were you yesterday). These questions can be seen as an attempt to get at the dichotomous nature of happiness as discussed earlier.

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 96 Psychometric measures of personal happiness and well-being most popular with psychological researchers also reflect this duality. Early measures such as the PANAS (Positive and Negative Affect Scale) attempted to gauge happiness as relative levels of positive and negative emotion (Kercher, 1992). Currently popular psychological measures of happiness tend to be more evaluative in nature, and include the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen & Griffen, 1985) and the Subjective Happiness Scale (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1997). In recent past, some researchers have attempted to tease apart the differences underlying the various concepts related to well-being, e.g., life satisfaction, positive and negative affect, self- esteem, and optimism. Using Campbell and Fiske’s (1959) multitrait-multimethod matrix analyses, Lucas, Diener and Suh (1996) found high discriminant validity for life satisfaction from positive and negative affect and also from optimism and self-esteem. Self-report questionnaire assessments have long been the mainstay of research in subjective well-being and happiness, as in many areas of psychological research. For some time now, researchers have been developing hybrid methods to measure happiness in the hope that such measures might increase validity. Although technically still a measure of self-report, Csikszentmihalyi (1997) introduced the Experience Sampling Method (ESM) in the 1970s. The ESM was novel in that it used electronic prompts to signal participants to record their immediate levels of happiness (or other variables such as motivation) rather than relying on retrospective reports. Csikszentmihalyi’s ESM utilizes random prompting to generate large quantities of data, that when compiled is assumed to be highly reliable. A recent attempt to improve upon self-report data in the measurement of happiness is described by Zhou (2011) as an approach that combines a happiness evaluation method developed by Ng with the day reconstruction method by Kahneman and associates. According to Zhou, the combined method moves happiness assessment toward more accuracy and standardization. Theories and Models of Happiness Psychological theories of happiness fall under three broad categories: need and goal satisfaction theories, process and activity theories, and genetic and personality predisposition theories. In need and goal satisfaction theories, the reduction of

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 98 sustainable happiness derives from three components in varying proportions. According to this model, roughly 10% of sustainable happiness is determined by life circumstances, 40% by intentional activity, and 50% is determined by a set point of biological and genetic forces. The idea that there is an element of stability in people’s levels of happiness (aside from influences due to life circumstances or the achievement of goals) due to personality dispositions is the fundamental premise of genetic and personality theory. The notion that there is a substantial genetic component to subjective well-being is strongly supported by scientific research findings (Headey & Wearing, 1989), especially in the study of twins. Tellegren et al. (1988, as cited in Diener, et al., 2009) found that in studies comparing both monozygotic and dizygotic twins raised together and apart, approximately 50% of the emotional reactivity of life circumstances can be explained by genes. Other studies (Diener et al, 2009) have correlated specific facets of personality traits to SWB; cheerfulness (a facet of extraversion) is strongly correlated with high levels of SWB, while depression (a facet of neuroticism) is strongly correlated to low levels of SWB. Dispositional cognitive styles such as optimism and hope are also linked to higher levels of happiness (Snyder et al., 1991, as cited in Diener et al., 2009; Scheier & Carver, 1993, as cited in Diener et al., 2009). Lyubomirsky’s tripartite model of sustainable happiness is congruent with the fact that psychological research often fails to confirm any single variable as causal, and that complex human experiences often result from the interaction of several dynamic variables. Diener’s decades-long research on SWB provides strong empirical evidence for this view, and notes that for the vast majority of people, the set point of happiness is slightly positive and is strongly influenced by temperament and externals such as cultural norms. Diener also notes that underlying an individual’s set point of happiness is the process of adaption; that people react strongly in the short term to good and bad events, but tend to habituate (adapt) over time and return to their original level (set point) of happiness (Diener, 2000). Adaptation theory has strongly influenced modern psychological theory about happiness since Bradburn and Campbell (1971, as cited in

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 99 Diener, Lucas & Scollon, 2006) described the process and coined the term the hedonic treadmill ; a theory that was scaffolded atop Helson’s automatic habituation model (1948, 1964, as cited in Diener et al., 2006). Automatic habituation is seen to be an adaptive process by which psychological systems react to deviations from one’s current adaptation level and allow constant influx of stimuli to diminish in impact. The notion of a hedonic treadmill has gained widespread appeal among many researchers, especially psychologists and behavioral economists, especially since early research with lottery winners and spinal cord victims seemed to confirm the view that humans can adapt to many events in life, and that these events do not exert a substantial long term effect on happiness levels (Brickman, Coates, & Janoff-Bulman, 1978). Brickman and colleagues posited that the theory of adaptation to happiness would explain why both a major favorable event such as winning a lottery and a major adverse event such as becoming permanently paralyzed ostensibly has little effect on SWB. The gist of the hedonic treadmill model is that the pleasures of success and the pain of failure all eventually fade away as we adapt back up or down to our stable set-point of happiness. A notable corollary to this idea is that people are poor at predicting their emotional state in the future (affective forecasting) and tend to overestimate how positive events like winning the lottery might affect their happiness, while at the same time ignoring other factors that will impact happiness levels (Kahneman, 2011). It should be noted that Headey and Wearing (1989) originally contended that the idea of the hedonic treadmill was subject to over-interpretation. Recent psychological research has confirmed this to be true, and is rapidly revising the traditional view of the hedonic treadmill, which is now known as hedonic adaptation (HA). Diener et al. (2006) note five important revisions to the established theory of HA, all of which are based on individual temperaments and differing underlying components of well-being, and Sheldon and Lyubomirsky (2012) suggests that “well-being decreases substantially after such adverse life events as unemployment, disability, divorce, and widowhood, and does not completely recover… and that strong negative events can throw people permanently ‘off kilter,’ exerting a durable negative influence on them such that they remain at a lower baseline than before” (p. 670).

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 101 personality trait of neuroticism (negatively correlated), it is also considered a cognitive explanatory style that can be learned (Seligman, 1990). Which raises the question that if depression (as learned helplessness) is a form of environmental influence and not a fixed genetic trait, does that hold true also for other facets of neuroticism such as anxiety and impulsivity? The answer to this question is related to the view that psychological traits have strong genetic components, but are not necessarily entirely stable over the lifespan. With respect to life circumstances, the literature now suggests that although external circumstances exert some influence on happiness, it is proportionally much less influential than the genetic set point or activities an individual pursue. Lyubomirsky (2008) has pegged it at about only 10% has a determinant of happiness. Research conducted and reviewed by Diener and associates (as cited in Boehm and Lyubomirsky, 2009) suggests that demographic factors related to life circumstances such as marriage, age, sex, culture, life events and income explain relatively little of the variance in happiness levels of individuals. Income has long been studied as a demographic correlate of subjective well- being. Although income has been correlated to subjective well-being both on a within- nation and between-nation basis, the consensus about to what degree income is correlated to well-being is rapidly eroding. Early economic research by Easterlin (1974) gave rise to an assumption popularly labeled the Easterlin Paradox , which stated that beyond a level of basic needs being met, a nation’s overall average well-being is not correlated to income levels. However, Sacks, Stevenson and Wolfer’s (2012) analysis of data from the Gallup World Poll reassesses the Easterlin Paradox, finding clear and convincing evidence that there is no satiation point for a nation’s wealth, and there is an unequivocal link between GDP per capita and SWB across countries. Regarding individual income levels, Diener et al., (2009) state that beyond a certain level, changes in personal income do not appear to influence an individual’s SWB significantly. In earlier research, Diener Horowitz and Edmonds (1985) found that multi-millionaire Americans are only negligibly happier than average Americans.

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 102 However, what an individual does with one’s income falls under the category of intentional activity, an area highlighted in previous discussion that holds significant potential for increasing personal happiness (Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013). Several recent studies confirm the view that money can buy happiness; so long as it is well- spent. West, Reed and Gildengorin (1998) found that when controlling for known correlates of depressive status, higher income was associated with lower depressive symptomology in older adults. More recently, Caprariello and Reis (2013) note that spending money on experiences, rather than on material possessions, makes people happier if the experiences are shared with others. This adds to the growing body of evidence that indicates that spending money on others, rather than oneself leads to higher levels of happiness (Aknin et al, 2013). Happiness is indeed a fuzzy concept— fraught with nuance, jangle about terminology, overlapping constructs and notions about what happiness entails. Yet if we can agree on anything—it is that no matter how ill-defined happiness is, as humans we desire more of it. We value and seek lives that are pleasant, engaged and meaningful. As humans, we prize the many correlates of happiness: more marital satisfaction, greater quality of our social lives, better job performance and higher incomes, greater physical health, longevity, and success (Diener, 2012; Rath & Harter, 2010). We recognize the many benefits and rewards that happiness bestows upon us, and as students of psychology, we also recognize our responsibility and intentionality toward happiness. We must also accept the complex interrelationship between correlational variables related to happiness. Many variables related to happiness appear to be bi-directional and bi-reciprocal in nature, and defy any clear attributions to causality. Lyubomirsky, King, and Diener (2005) state this position succinctly in their title page as they examine the juxtaposition of correlational evidence in the relationship between success and happiness and ponder: Does Happiness Lead to Success? This may well be the seminal question in our scientific inquiry regarding well-being: does happiness lead to success, or does success lead to happiness? The answer appears to be— yes!

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 104 Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with life scale. Journal Of Personality Assessment , 49 (1), 71-75. Diener, E. Horowitz, J. & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Happiness of the very wealthy. Social Indicators Research, 16, 263 - 274. Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. N. (2009). Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being_. In The science of well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener_ (E. Diener (Ed.), (pp. 103-118). New York, NY US: Springer Science + Business Media. Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2009). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and life satisfaction. In S. J. Lopez, & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), (pp. 187- 194). New York, NY US: Oxford University Press. Easterlin, R. A. (1974). “Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence, P. A. David and M. W.Reder ( Eds.) , Nations and households in economic growth: Essays in honour of Moses Abramovitz. New York, NY: Academic Press. Freud, S. (1950). Beyond the pleasure principle (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York, NY: Liveright. (Original work published 1920). Gallup. (n.d.). [No format]. Retrieved online from http://www.well-beingindex.com/ and http://www.gallup.com/video/106357/introducing-gallup-world-poll.aspx Happy- A Documentary Trailer. (n.d.). [Video]. Retrieved online from http://vimeo.com/ Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 (4), 731-739. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.4. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York : Farrar, Straus and Giroux Kercher, K. (1992). Assessing subjective well-being in the old-old: The PANAS as a measure of orthogonal dimensions of positive and negative affect. Research on Aging, 14 (2), 131-168. doi:10.1177/ Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1996). Discriminant validity of well-being measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (3), 616-628.

Counseling and Wellness: A Professional Counseling Journal 105 Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). The how of happiness: A scientific approach to getting the life you want. New York: Penguin Press. Lyubomirsky, S. & Dickerhoof, R. (2006). Subjective well-being. In J. Worrell & C.D. Goodheart (Eds.). Handbook of girls' and women's psychological health: Gender and well-being across the lifespan (166-174). N Y: Oxford University Press. Lyubomirsky, S. King, L. & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131 (6), 803-855. Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). How do simple positive activities increase well-being? Current Directions In Psychological Science, 22(1), 57-62. Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research , 46(2), 137 - 155. doi:10.1023/A: Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd^ ed.). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company Nelson, L. D., & Meyvis, T. (2008). Interrupted consumption: Disrupting adaptation to hedonic experiences. Journal of Marketing Research, 45 (6), 654-664. doi:10.1509/jmkr.45.6. Peterson, C., & Park, N. (2009). Classifying and measuring strengths of character. In S. J.Lopez, C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed.) (pp. 25-33). New York, NY US: Oxford University Press. Rath, T. & Harter, J. (2010). Well being: The five essential elements. New York: The Gallup Press. Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company. Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2012). The new stylized facts about income and subjective well-being. Emotion, 12 (6), 1181-1187. doi:10.1037/a Schoenberg, N. (2011). Happiness, meet science. Chicago Tribune. Retrieved online from http://articles.chicagotribune.com/2011- 04 - 12/features/ct-mag-tribu- happiness-20110410_1_happiness-psychology-professor-positive-psychology Scientific American: The Science of Happiness (n.d.) [Report]. Retrieved online from http://www.scientificamerican.com/report.cfm?id=science-of-happiness