Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

The Reward Theory of Attraction, Study notes of Psychology

Interpersonal attraction as a function of type of favorable and unfavorable evaluation in explain reward theory by Jay Hewitt from university of Missouri Kansas.

Typology: Study notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 03/31/2022

nicoline
nicoline 🇺🇸

4.6

(12)

277 documents

1 / 2

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
Interpersonal attraction
as
a function
of
the type
of
favorable and
unfavorable evaluation
JAY
HEWITT
University
of
Missouri·Kansas
City,
Kansas
City,
Mo.
64110
The
Ss received
personal
evaluations
from
three
confederates
and
then
rated
their
liking for
each
individuaL
Favorable
evaluations
produced
more
attraction
than
unfavorable
evaluations
regardless
of
whether
they
were
consistent
or
inconsistent
with
S's
perception
of
himself-
The
specific
content
of
the
evaluation
was
more
important
when
the
evaluation
was
unfavorable
than
when
it
was favorable.
Different
types
of
favorable
evaluation
were
not
associated
with
differential
degrees
of
liking.
Unfavorable
evaluations
that
were
inconsistent
with
S's
pereption
of
himself
produced
greater
hostility
than
those
that
were
consistent
with
S's
perception
of
himself-
According
to
the
reward
theory
of
interpersonal
attraction,
people
are
attracted
to
those
who
reward
or
reinforce
them.
One
type
of
reward
a
person
can
receive
from
others
is
esteem.
People
tend
to
be
more
attracted
to
those
who
evaluate
them
favorably
than
to
those
who
evaluate
them
unfavorably
(Aronson
&
Worchel,
1966;
Backman
&
Secord,
1959;
Byrne
&
Griffitt,
1966).
As
Aristotle
has
pointed
out:
"We
feel
friendly
toward
those
who
praise
such
good
qualities
as we possess
...
toward
those
who
are
not
evilspeakers
and
who
are
aware
of
neither
their
nieghbor's
bad
points
nor
our
own,
but
of
our
good
ones
only
([
translated
1924,
p.138]."
Although
reward
theory
has
been
shown
to
be
a viable
model
of
interpersonal
attraction,
reward
theory
does
not
s·pecify
the
type
of
favorable
evaluation
that
will
lead
to
the
greatest
attraction
or
the
type
of
unfavorable
evaluation
that
will
lead
to
the
greatest
hostility.
According
to
the
cognitive
consistency
theory
of
interpersonal
attraction,
the
attraction
of
one
person
(P)
to
another
person
(0)
is
a
function
of
the
similarity
between
O's
perception
of
P
and
P's
perception
of
himself
(Deutsch
&
Solomon,
1959).
People
are
most
attracted
to
those
who
see
them
as
they
see themselves.
Some
support
for
this
proposition
was
obtained
in a
study
by
Hewitt
(1969).
Ss received
evaluations
that
were
either
favorable
or
unfavorable
and
either
accurate
or
relatively
inaccurate.
Accurate
evaluations
consisted
of
traits
the
S
had
previously
used
to
describe
himself,
while
inaccurate
evaluations
consisted
of
traits
another
S
had
employed
to
describe
himself.
Accurate
evaluations
tended
to
generate
higher
attraction
ratings
than
inaccurate
evaluations.
The
present
study
was
designed
to
examine
the
generality
of
the
Psychon.
Sci.,
1971,
VoL
22
(4)
relationship
between
accuracy
of
evaluation
and
liking.
If
a
person
were
told
that
he
possessed
those
characteristics
he
was striving
to
possess
(but
currently
did
not
possess)
or
did
not
possess
the
undesirable
characteristics
he
felt
he
did
possess,
this
might
serve
to
enhance
his
perception
of
himself
and,
as a
consequence,
lead
to
a relatively high
degree
of
attraction
toward
the
evaluator.
Even
though
such
an
evaluation
would
be
inaccurate,
it
might
lead
to
more
attraction
than
an
evaluation
that
merely
reaffirmed
the
S's
perception
of
his desirable
characteristics.
Conversely,
there
may
be
some
situations
in
which
accurate
unfavorable
evaluations
would
generate
more
hostility
than
inaccurate
unfavorable
evaluations.
If
a
person
were
told
that
he
did
not
possess
those
characteristics
he
was
striving
to
possess,
this
might
be
a
source
of
need
frustration
and,
as a
consequence,
might
generate
a
high
degree
of
animosity
toward
the
evaluator.
DESIGN
Each
S received
three
evaluations
of
himself,
one
from
each
of
three
different
confederates,
and
then
rated
his liking for
each
evaluator.
A 2
by
3
design was
employed,
with
each
evaluation
being
either
favorable
or
unfavorable
and
based
on
(1)
the
S's
desirable
characteristics,
(2)
the
S's
ideal
characteristics,
or
(3)
the
S's
undesirable
characteristics.
Each
S was
told
that
he
either
possessed
or
did
not
possess
the
traits
he
had
employed
to
describe
his
desirable
characteristics,
that
he
either
possessed
or
did
not
possess
the
traits
he
had
employed
to
describe
his ideal
characteristics
(the
traits
he
would
most
like
to
possess
but
currently
did
not
possess),
and
that
he
either
possessed
or
did
not
possess
the
traits
he
had
employed
to
describe
his
undesirable
characteristics.
SUBJECTS
The
Ss
consisted
of
30
males
recruited
from
an
introductory
psychology
course.
Half
of
the
Ss
were
assigned
to
the
"favorable
evaluation"
condition,
the
remaining
to
the
"unfavorable
evaluation"
condition.
PROCEDURE
Upon
arriving for
the
experiment,
the
S was
seated
in a small
soundproof
cubicle
and
handed
three
lists
of
adjectives.
On
the
first list,
the
S was
told
to
put
a
check
mark
beside
the
five
adjectives
that
best
described
his
desirable
characteristics,
on
the
second
list, a
check
mark
beside
the
five
adjectives
that
best
described
the
characteristics
he
would
most
like
to
possess
(but
currently
did
not
possess),
and
on
the
third
list, a
check
mark
beside
the
five
adjectives
that
best
described
his
undesirable
characteristics.
The
S was
then
informed
that
there
were
three
other
people
participating
in
the
experiment
(Ss A, B,
and
C),
that
each
S was
located
in a
different
room,
and
that
all
rooms
were
connected
via
intercom.
(In
reality,
A, B,
and
C
were
confederates.
)
After
the
initial
instructions
had
been
given,
the
S
was
handed
a
sheet
which
summarized
the
procedure.
The
other
people
participating
in
the
experiment
would
each
be
given a list
on
which
there
were
five
checked
adjectives
(the
S's
desirable,
ideal,
or
undesirable
characteristics).
The
S
would
then
be
required
to
answer
four
questions
about
himself.
A, B,
and
C
would
be
able
to
hear
these
answers
over
their
intercom,
after
which
they
would
be
asked
to
give
their
impression
of
the
S.
The
actual
procedure
was
then
carried
out.
Each
confederate
was
given a list
on
which
there
were
five
checked
adjectives.
The
S was
then
asked
to
describe
his
interests,
his
goals in life,
the
kind
of
people
he
liked
and
disliked,
and
his
personal
problems.
After
this
information
had
been
delivered (via
intercom)
to
A, B,
and
C,
Person
A
was
asked
to
go
through
each
of
the
checked
adjectives
on
his list.
After
naming
each
trait,
Person
A was
asked
to
say
whether
or
not
that
trait
characterized
or
did
not
characterize
the
S.
When
A was
finished,
similar
instructions
were
given
to
B
and
then
to
C.
The
S was
able
to
hear
these
comments
over
his
intercom.
When
all
three
evaluations
had
been
delivered,
the
S was
asked
to
rate
his liking
for
A,
B,
and
C
on
a
scale ranging
from
+10
(strong
liking)
to
-10
(strong
dislike),
after
which
he
was
debriefed
and
then
dismissed.
EV
ALUATIONS
In
the
favorable
evaluation
condition,
Ss
were
told
that
they
possessed
the
two
sets
of
desirable
197
pf2

Partial preview of the text

Download The Reward Theory of Attraction and more Study notes Psychology in PDF only on Docsity!

Interpersonal attraction as a function

of the type of favorable and

unfavorable evaluation

JAY HEWITT

University of Missouri·Kansas City, Kansas City, Mo. 64110

The Ss received personal evaluations from three confederates and then rated their liking for each individuaL Favorable evaluations produced more attraction than unfavorable evaluations regardless of whether they were consistent or inconsistent with S's perception of himself- The specific content of the evaluation was more important when the evaluation was unfavorable than when it was favorable. Different types of favorable evaluation were not associated with differential degrees of liking. Unfavorable evaluations that were inconsistent with S's pereption of himself produced greater hostility than those that were consistent with S's perception of himself-

According to the reward theory of interpersonal attraction, people are attracted to those who reward or reinforce them. One type of reward a person can receive from others is esteem. People tend to be more attracted to those who evaluate them favorably than to those who evaluate them unfavorably (Aronson & Worchel, 1966; Backman & Secord, 1959; Byrne & Griffitt, 1966). As Aristotle has pointed out: "We feel friendly toward those who praise such good qualities as we possess ... toward those who are not evilspeakers and who are aware of neither their nieghbor's bad points nor our own, but of our good ones only ([ translated 1924, p.138]." Although reward theory has been shown to be a viable model of interpersonal attraction, reward theory does not s·pecify the type of favorable evaluation that will lead to the greatest attraction or the type of unfavorable evaluation that will lead to the greatest hostility. According to the cognitive consistency theory of interpersonal attraction, the attraction of one person (P) to another person (0) is a function of the similarity between O's perception of P and P's perception of himself (Deutsch & Solomon, 1959). People are most attracted to those who see them as they see themselves. Some support for this proposition was obtained in a study by Hewitt (1969). Ss received evaluations that were either favorable or unfavorable and either accurate or relatively inaccurate. Accurate evaluations consisted of traits the S had previously used to describe himself, while inaccurate evaluations consisted of traits another S had employed to describe himself. Accurate evaluations tended to generate higher attraction ratings than inaccurate evaluations. The present study was designed to examine the generality of the

Psychon. Sci., 1971, VoL 22 (4)

relationship between accuracy of evaluation and liking. If a person were told that he possessed those characteristics he was striving to possess (but currently did not possess) or did not possess the undesirable characteristics he felt he did possess, this might serve to enhance his perception of himself and, as a consequence, lead to a relatively high degree of attraction toward the evaluator. Even though such an evaluation would be inaccurate, it might lead to more attraction than an evaluation that merely reaffirmed the S's perception of his desirable characteristics. Conversely, there may be some situations in which accurate unfavorable evaluations would generate more hostility than inaccurate unfavorable evaluations. If a person were told that he did not possess those characteristics he was striving to possess, this might be a source of need frustration and, as a consequence, might generate a high degree of animosity toward the evaluator. DESIGN Each S received three evaluations of himself, one from each of three different confederates, and then rated his liking for each evaluator. A 2 by 3 design was employed, with each evaluation being either favorable or unfavorable and based on (1) the S's desirable characteristics, (2) the S's ideal characteristics, or (3) the S's undesirable characteristics. Each S was told that he either possessed or did not possess the traits he had employed to describe his desirable characteristics, that he either possessed or did not possess the traits he had employed to describe his ideal characteristics (the traits he would most like to possess but currently did not possess), and that he either possessed or did not possess the traits he had employed to describe his undesirable characteristics.

SUBJECTS

The Ss consisted of 30 males recruited from an introductory psychology course. Half of the Ss were assigned to the "favorable evaluation" condition, the remaining to the "unfavorable evaluation" condition. PROCEDURE Upon arriving for the experiment, the S was seated in a small soundproof cubicle and handed three lists of adjectives. On the first list, the S was told to put a check mark beside the five adjectives that best described his desirable characteristics, on the second list, a check mark beside the five adjectives that best described the characteristics he would most like to possess (but currently did not possess), and on the third list, a check mark beside the five adjectives that best described his undesirable characteristics. The S was then informed that there were three other people participating in the experiment (Ss A, B, and C), that each S was located in a different room, and that all rooms were connected via intercom. (In reality, A, B, and C were confederates. ) After the initial instructions had been given, the S was handed a sheet which summarized the procedure. The other people participating in the experiment would each be given a list on which there were five checked adjectives (the S's desirable, ideal, or undesirable characteristics). The S would then be required to answer four questions about himself. A, B, and C would be able to hear these answers over their intercom, after which they would be asked to give their impression of the S. The actual procedure was then carried out. Each confederate was given a list on which there were five checked adjectives. The S was then asked to describe his interests, his goals in life, the kind of people he liked and disliked, and his personal problems. After this information had been delivered (via intercom) to A, B, and C, Person A was asked to go through each of the checked adjectives on his list. After naming each trait, Person A was asked to say whether or not that trait characterized or did not characterize the S. When A was finished, similar instructions were given to B and then to C. The S was able to hear these comments over his intercom. When all three evaluations had been delivered, the S was asked to rate his liking for A, B, and C on a scale ranging from +10 (strong liking) to -10 (strong dislike), after which he was debriefed and then dismissed. EV ALUATIONS In the favorable evaluation condition, Ss were told that they possessed the two sets of desirable

Table 1 Description of Evaluations

Table 2 Mean Liking for the Evaluator Evaluation (^) Evaluation "Does (^) "Does Not List Referent Favorable Unfavorable List (^) Referent Possess Traits" (^) Possess Traits"

Desirable Traits Favorable S Ascribes to Self (^) Accurate II Desirable Traits Favorable Ideal Inaccurate III Undesirable Traits Unfavorable S Ascribes to Self (^) Accurate

characteristics and did not possess the set of undesirable characteristics. One confederate (A, B, or C) felt that the S possessed four of the traits on List 1 (traits used by S to describe his desirable characteristics). One confederate felt that S possessed four of the traits on List 2 (traits the S would most like to possess but currently did not possess), and one confederate felt that S did not possess four of the traits on List 3 (traits used by the S to describe his undesirable characteristics). Confederate A was always unsure as to whether or not the S possessed the first checked adjective on his list, B was unsure about the third checked adjective on his list, and C was unsure about the fourth checked adjective on his list. The evaluations in the unfavorable evaluation condition were the mirror image of those in the favorable evaluation condition-Ss were told that they did not possess the two sets of desirable characteristics and did possess the set of undesirable characteristics. A more complete description of these evaluations is given in Table 1. RESULTS Favorable evaluations produced more attraction than unfavorable evaluations. The specific content of the evaluation was relatively unimportant when the evaluation was favorable but was a relevant variable when the evaluation was unfavorable. The inaccurate unfavorable evaluation generated more hostility than the two unfavorable evaluations that were consistent with the S's perception of himself. The means are presented in Table 2.

A Type I analysis of variance was carried out on the data (Lindquist, 1953, p. 267). Favorable evaluations generated more attraction than unfavorable evaluations (F = 8.61;

df = 2/56; p < .01), and there was a

Desirable Traits 3.87* -2.13** Unfavorable S Ascribes to Self Inaccurate II Desirable Traits 3.93^ 2.00^ * Unfavorable Ideal Accurate (^) III Undesirable Traits 3.87** 2.27* Favorable S^ Ascribes^ to^ Self Inaccurate *"Does possess traits"; **"Does not possess traits."

significant interaction between the type of evaluation (favorable vs unfavorable) and the list of checked adjectives on which the evaluation was based (F = 13.82; df = 1/28;

p < .001). The specific content of the

evaluation appeared to be more important when the evaluation was unfavorable than when it was favorable. Different types of favorable evaluations were not associated with differential degrees of liking. Content was an important variable when the evaluation was unfavorable, with the inaccurate unfavol"\lble evaluation generating more hostility than either of the two accurate unfavorable evaluations (t = 3.86 and 3.22; df = 14; p < .01, for both). Ss were less attracted to someone who said they did not possess the desirable characteristics they felt they did possess than (1) someone who said they did not possess their ideal characteristics and (2) someone who said they did possess their undesirable characteristics.

DISCUSSION

The results were generally consistent with reward theory. Favorable evaluations produced more attraction than unfavorable evaluations. Contrary to the cognitive consistency theory of interpersonal attraction, accuracy was not a relevant variable when the evaluation was favorable. Favorable evaluations appeared to generate a high degree of attraction with the specific content of the evaluation being relatively Unimportant. People apparently like to be flattered and tend to accept such a communication regardless of whether the flattery is genuine or totally inaccurate. Consistency theory also failed to receive support when accuracy and favorability were opposed to one another. Accurate unfavorable evaluations did not generate more

attraction than inaccurate favorable evaluations. In fact, just the reverse tended to be true. Pointing out a person's undesirable characteristics would appear to lead to less attraction than telling him that he had desirable characteristics which in fact he did not possess. When reward and cognitive consistency theory make differential predictions, reward theory appears to be a more viable model of interpersonal attraction. Cognitive consistency theory was supported when the evaluations were unfavorable. As in the Hewitt (1969) study, inaccurate unfavorable evaluations generated more hostility than accurate unfavorable evaluations. When such an evaluation is consistent with the S's perception of himself, the communication is apparently accepted, and the S remains relatively neutral toward the communicator. When the evaluation is inconsistent with the S's perception of himself, however, it would appear that both the communication and the communicator tend to be rejected.

REFERENCES ARISTOTLE. The works of Aristotle translated into English. Vol. II. Oxford: Clarenden Press, 1924. ARONSON, E., & WORCHEL, P. Similarity versus liking as detel'minants of interpersonal attractiveness. Psychonomic Sciencc, 1966, 5, 157-158. BACKMAN, C. W., & SECORD, P. F. The effect of perceived liking on interpersonal attraction. Human Relations, 1959, 12, 379-384. BYRNE, D .. & GRIFFITT, W. Similarity versus liking: A clarification. Psychonomic Science, 1966. 6, 295-296. DEUTSCH, M., & SOLOMON. L. Reactions to evaluations by others as influenced by self-evaluations. Sociometry. 1959, 22, 93-112. HEWITT, J. Interpersonal attraction as a function of the accuracy of personal evaluations. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 17,95-96. LINDQUIST, E. F. Design and analysis of experiments in psychology and education. Cambridge, Mass: Houghton-Mifflin, 1953.

Psychon. Sci., 1971, Vol. 22 (4)